5
Successful Solution Discovery and Cognitive Flexibility

Evelyne CLÉMENT

Paragraphe, CY Cergy Paris University, Gennevilliers, France

5.1. Introduction

In daily life, at school or at work, cognitive flexibility is required in order to adapt and adjust our behavior to a changing environment. In addition, developing new skills and knowledge, or transferring and adapting prior knowledge in response to the constraints of the situation, also demonstrates flexibility. Finally, and specifically in problem solving, discovering the solution most often requires changing our perspective, that is to say changing our representation of the situation and thus demonstrating representational or conceptual flexibility.

In this chapter, after presenting what constitutes a problem situation – by referring to the role of knowledge about the world and the transfer processes involved in finding solutions – we describe how the concept of flexibility is understood in the fields of educational science and cognitive science, respectively. We then present the results of recent research on the links between flexibility, creativity and academic performance. We discuss the relevance, both from a fundamental point of view and in terms of educational implications, of developing programs to stimulate flexibility and creative thinking at school.

5.2. Cognitive flexibility in problem solving

5.2.1. What is defined as a problem?

First of all, it is important to know what is considered a problem situation. Or, to put it another way, in the fields of cognitive psychology and educational sciences, what is defined as a problem? Generally speaking, this notion covers a wide variety of situations in which we have to reach a goal, but we do not know how to achieve it. A situation is thus defined as a problem when it is new, non-routine; in other words, a situation is described as a problem when knowledge and known procedures cannot be directly applied. In this way, some authors in the field of mathematics education differentiate between so-called “routine” and “non-routine” problems. To address routine problems, strategies and procedures learned in previous exercises can be applied to find the solution. For non-routine problems, flexible and creative thinking is required in order to find a way to resolve the problem (see, for example, Elia et al. (2009)).

As can be noted, it is not so much the objective properties of a situation that define it as a problem, but rather the knowledge and expertise we have about the situation (Novick 1988; Krems 1995). As defined above, a “problem” is any situation that is non-routine. In this way, we consider that any problem-solving situation is a learning situation. Conversely, any learning situation is by nature a problem-solving situation, in which our prior knowledge can be used but will require transformation, thus showing cognitive flexibility to master the new concepts involved in the situation.

In learning situations, and in problem solving in particular, when faced with a new situation, we try, consciously or not, to apply familiar knowledge and procedures. It is on the basis of the known that we make sense of the new. Although learning by analogy is one of the pillars of learning, it is possible that prior knowledge may make it difficult to find a solution in the new situation. In this case, it will be necessary to change our representation on the problem and recode the situation in order to select the properties that are not salient at first glance, but that are relevant to solve the problem.

5.2.2. Familiar knowledge about the world, problem solving and transfer

5.2.2.1. The crucial role of prior knowledge

A large body of research shows that our experience and knowledge about the world play a crucial role in problem solving (Bassok et al. 1995). Indeed, the role of prior knowledge in conceptual acquisition and development has been demonstrated in different domains throughout its lifetime (see, for example, Vosniadou and Ortony (1989)). For example, in problem solving, Clément and Richard (1997) show that familiar knowledge about the actions that allow a change of state could explain the great differences in difficulty between isomorphic problems that share the same solution principle, but whose contexts require knowledge about the change of state that may or may not be compatible with the solution. To test the role of familiar knowledge evoked by the problem statements, different isomorphic versions of the same problem were used. In some versions (the change-of-place problems), the adequate point of view of the action was emphasized, while in others (the change-of-size problems), the point of view of the action evoked by the statements had to be abandoned in order to discover the most efficient solution path. Results have shown that in the change-of-size problems, in which change is conceived as a continuous process of growth, adults take longer to find the solution and/or perform more actions and rule violations than in the change-of-place versions. In the change-of-size versions, discovering the most efficient solution path has been interpreted by Clement (2009) as the expression of cognitive flexibility, because the first representation has to be abandoned and a new point of view on the problem has to be adopted.

Understanding a situation on the basis of prior knowledge is a general process of learning by analogy. It is defined by some authors as being at the heart of thinking (Hofstadter and Sander 2013). At school, for example, making use of students’ prior knowledge is one way to teach new concepts. In this way, teaching new concepts is often planned around this familiar knowledge, acquired through experience. Bassok et al. (1998) have demonstrated this by conducting an analysis of mathematics textbooks used in America across grades one to eight. The authors report that the semantic relationships between the objects in the problem statements are very strongly correlated with the arithmetic operations to be applied on these objects. They note that in 97% of addition problem statements, the objects to be added are objects belonging to the same category. For example, they would be red marbles and blue marbles, but never cookies and baskets. Similarly, 94% of problems that require division involve objects that are functionally related. In this case, the objects involved might be cookies and baskets, but never red marbles and blue marbles.

Furthermore, when students are asked to design simple addition or division problem statements involving pairs of objects – for example, tulips and daisies or tulips and vases – students tend to construct statements in which the mathematical structure and the semantic structure (i.e. the relationships between the objects) are compatible with the pairs involving objects of the same category. When tulips and daisies are involved, it evokes addition problem statements. With pairs of objects that have a functional relationship (e.g. containing, as with the pairing of tulips and vases), division problems are proposed by the majority of students (see Experiment 1 in Bassok et al. (1998)). These results suggest that participants who have been taught simple arithmetic problems throughout their school careers apply their mathematical knowledge in ways that are consistent with their familiar knowledge about the relationships between objects in daily life. This semantic alignment, which is guided by our knowledge about the world, is observed in both adults and children (see, for example, Martin et al. (2005)). In fact, in adults this process is highly automatic (Bassok et al. 2008).

Building on students’ familiar knowledge in order to enable them to understand new concepts is certainly a necessary step in learning. However, it may not be sufficient to lead students to a deep understanding and mastery of the concepts taught at school. In their experiment, Moss and Case (1999) illustrate this phenomenon. They demonstrate how educational initiatives that can be put into practice at school have a significant influence on the development of mathematical knowledge. In their study, the authors designed a program for Canadian fourth graders aged 9–10 years. In order to improve students’ understanding and performance in solving problems involving rational numbers, the authors conceived an experimental program by reorganizing the order in which rational numbers were traditionally taught. To this end, and in contrast to Canadian curricula which advocated the introduction of decimals as an alternative way of representing fractions, the learning sequence consisted of first introducing percentages, then decimals, and finally mathematical sequences on fractions. Moss and Case report that children who received this type of instruction were less likely to mistakenly transfer strategies that were relevant for whole numbers but ineffective for rational numbers.

This body of work suggests that the encoding of concepts in memory and the links that may be made between different domains are crucial steps in learning and mastering mathematical concepts. It shows how the design of learning sequences can affect the quality of learning. This also demonstrates the importance of considering the role of prior and familiar knowledge in curriculum design; besides it demonstrates that the knowledge transmitted and acquired at school can have a longer term impact on the scientific understanding of mathematical concepts.

5.2.2.2. Positive, negative or no transfer

As described, our knowledge about the world can facilitate, hinder or prevent the discovery of a successful solution to a problem. Indeed, the encoding of a new problem very often consists of trying to apply what we have learned when solving previous problems. This can lead to the transfer of inadequate solutions and strategies that are learned in previous situations and that, by analogy, are perceived by the solver as similar to the new situation. In fact, the way the situation is encoded and categorized determines if the transfer will be positive, negative or if there will be no transfer.

A large body of studies carried out in the field of transfer between analogs – that is, problems sharing the same solution principle, but not systematically the same surface similarities such as characters, places, temporality, and so on – shows that the ease or difficulty encountered by the solver lie in the way in which the new problem (the target) will be encoded and categorized on the basis of perceived similarities between the situations (see Chapters 7 and 8). Transfer will be positive when problems that share the same solution principle – that is to say the same abstract structure – are perceived by the solver as similar. In the same manner, transfer will be negative when problems that do not share the same abstract structure are perceived as similar. Finally, there will be no transfer when problems sharing the same solution principle are perceived as dissimilar. In other words, when the source problem and the target problem evoke similar structures, there will be transfer. When the source and the target evoke different structures, the situations are perceived as different and the solver does not use what they have previously learned to solve the new problem. To successfully transfer and solve a new problem, it is necessary to categorize the known problem and the new problem on the basis of their structural similarity, regardless of their surface similarity. In many cases, this means changing our point-of-view on the problem, in other words, being flexible.

Concerning the negative transfer between problems, the harmful effect of past experience has been well described since the beginning of the 20th century. This is what the Gestalt Theory described as a phenomenon of mechanization of thought. For example, the deleterious effects of experience on the solution discovery were highlighted in the famous jars problems created by Luchins (1939, 1942). In these problems, some quantities of liquid must be transferred from one jar to another to obtain a new quantity. Luchins reports that adult participants who solved an initial set of problems by applying the same sequence of operations between three jars do not spontaneously discover a simpler solution in which the transfer between only two jars is necessary and sufficient. Although this procedure is simpler than the first, the majority of adults continue to apply the known procedure involving the three jars at hand.

Similarly, studies with young children have shown how solving a first problem (the source problem) can negatively affect solving a new problem (the target problem). For example, Chen and Daehler (1989) report the difficulties of 6-year-olds in not blindly applying a previously taught solution to new problems whose taught solution principle proves ineffective. The authors used two problems with the following two solution principles. In one problem, the solution consists of recovering an object floating in a container of water, but which is not directly accessible. To recover the object, simply pour water into the container to raise the level and catch the object. The solution to the second problem of catching a distant object that is not directly accessible is to connect two objects together to extend the arm and catch the object. As previously noted, negative transfer occurs when the source and target problems do not share the same solution principle. That is, like the Luchins’ adults participants, children blindly apply the procedure they learned in the first problem. They try in vain to reproduce the learned solution and do not spontaneously discover the solution of the second problem. Furthermore, it turns out that training children to abstract the solution principle from the source problem is beneficial when the source problem shares the same solution principle as the new target problem. However, when faced with a new problem that does not share the same solution principle, this training does not allow children to identify the irrelevance of the solution principle they were taught.

Concerning the absence of transfer between problems – a finding often reported by teachers – it appears that transfer is not systematic, even in the case where several examples are offered to the solver in order to allow them to categorize the problems according to solution principle and thus transfer the solution from one problem to another. The simple exposition and resolution of isomorphic problems is not sufficient to categorize the examples as belonging to the same problem pattern. In addition, studies have shown that the transfer between problems belonging to different domains is not spontaneous if the time between learning and transfer is long or if the context changes (Spencer and Weisberg 1986; Holyoak 2005). Transfer only occurs by encouraging participants to identify the abstract structure of problems. For example, Catrambone and Holyoak (1989) asked students to study, compare and solve isomorphic problems involving a principle of division and convergence of forces on a target to be destroyed (e.g. a medical problem of destroying a tumor by preserving the patient’s healthy tissues, or a military problem of destroying a fortress by dividing troops into small groups, in order to avoid blowing up the mined roads leading to the fortress).

To encourage the students to abstract the deep structure of the problems, the experimenters asked them to answer a series of very detailed questions about the problems. Then, they were asked to solve a third problem. The solution principle common to all three problems was then presented to them. Finally, one week later, the same students were invited to participate in a new experiment before solving the tumor problem inspired by Duncker’s problem (1945). The authors report that more than 80% of the participants spontaneously discovered the solution of simultaneously delivering converging low-intensity radiation to destroy the tumor and preserve healthy tissue. According to Holyoak (2005), the expertise developed by students in this experiment allowed them to elaborate and access in memory, an abstract schema evoked by a new problem that shares the same structure. Similarly, encouraging students to compare the structure of problems, regardless of their superficial similarity, has also proven effective with young children (see, for example, Gamo et al. (2010)).

We can take advantage of such results in the field of academic learning. In this way, encouraging students to identify, beyond their superficial similarities, the abstract and deep structure of situations is certainly a teaching approach to be developed. It is one of the ways of stimulating cognitive flexibility that involves changing perspective on the problem and selecting the relevant cues in the situation. Thus, in these situations, stimulating cognitive flexibility consists of training the learners to abstract, beneath or beyond the form, the profound structure of the problem. In summary, work on the processes involved in transfer shows that effective and flexible knowledge transfer requires deep understanding and encoding of situations, but also recognition of the context in which that knowledge is applicable. Selecting the relevant cues in the situation and identifying the contexts in which the knowledge is applicable is evidence of cognitive flexibility as we define it.

As Brown and Campione (1984) point out:

A major barrier to flexible learning is often not a lack of transfer between different fields of knowledge, but rather inappropriate transfer from one field to another. Successful learning involves knowing when, where, and what to transfer rather than blindly applying knowledge (p. 185).

Thus, access to relevant knowledge in memory and its appropriate use to find the solution are heavily guided by the context of the new problem to be solved. It is what is called the “evocation processes” and the “problem content effects“. These aspects are developed in more detail in Chapters 7 and 8. Indeed, much more than the surface or perceptual objective similarities shared by the situations, the transfer between problems depends on the way these similitudes are evoked by the content of the problems and encoded by the solver (Kotovsky and Fallside 1989; Bassok et al. 1995). Indeed, it appears that perceiving similarities and analogies is one of the fundamental aspects of human cognition. It is in this sense that Vosniadou and Ortony (1989) describe this capacity as crucial in recognition, categorization and learning: perceiving a structural analogy between situations plays a determining role in the discovery of solutions, just as in the case of scientific discoveries or creative thinking. In a related area to problem solving, recent research on spontaneous analogies shows that when situations are familiar (in this case short scenarios describing everyday situations), they preferentially evoke in memory, situations that only share structural similarities and no surface similarities (Raynal et al. 2020). When situations are familiar, individuals tend to favor structural similarities and neglect surface similarities. On the other hand, when situations are unfamiliar, then individuals will tend to favor superficial cues. This result supports the idea that in the school domain, when new notions are sometimes difficult to understand, training students to categorize situations on the basis of their deep structure is a powerful way to allow them to construct new knowledge on the basis of relevant cues. In sum, this body of research shows how an individual’s knowledge, level of familiarity with the problem and problem context can contribute to the flexible expression of problem-solving behaviors and, more generally, are key determinants of successful learning and conceptual development.

So far, we have discussed the role played by the knowledge activated by the problem context and the expertise of the solver in the discovery and flexible transfer. A situation is a problem when solution discovery requires understanding the deep structure of the problem (the principles of the solution), while neglecting superficial similarities. In this context, the successful solution discovery consists of recoding the situation. It involves cognitive flexibility, which allows us to consider the situation from a new perspective, to modify our initial representation of the goal and to select the relevant information to achieve the goal (Clément 2009).

In order to introduce the topic of this chapter, we have focused on problem solving and the processes at work in the discovery and transfer of solutions. We have deliberately chosen to discuss the notion of cognitive flexibility from the perspective of the work carried out in cognitive psychology in the field of problem solving, as if the definition of flexibility was a consensus in the literature. In the following section, we will present the different meanings of this notion, the nuances and the differences that can be found according to the fields of research, although recently, links may be established between approaches developed in the field of education, neuropsychology, cognitive psychology or developmental psychology (for a review of the issues, see Clément (forthcoming)).

5.2.3. Strategic flexibility, representational flexibility and solution discovery

The term flexibility has different meanings in the scientific literature on problem solving, particularly in the fields of educational research and psychology. Indeed, some emphasize strategic flexibility, others representational flexibility, and others switching, from one stimulus to another or from one task to another.

In this section, we focus on research conducted in the areas of mathematics education and cognitive psychology of problem solving (for a review of the different meanings of the term, especially in neuropsychology, see Clément (2009); Clément (forthcoming); also see Chapter 2 of this book for a developmental approach).

5.2.3.1. Research in mathematics education

In mathematics education research, flexibility is defined in a somewhat different sense than that defined by researchers in cognitive or developmental psychology. Thus, in studies conducted in mathematics didactics, flexibility is often understood as either the ability to change strategies flexibly in order to solve the problem as quickly as possible (Kilpatrick et al. 2001; Baroody and Dowker 2003; Verschaffel et al. 2007; Torbeyns et al. 2018), or the ability to choose the teacher-provided representational format that is the most appropriate for solving the problem (Acevedo Nistal et al. 2012).

Strategic flexibility, as defined by influential authors in mathematics teaching, is to be understood in the more general framework of the cognitive model developed by Lemaire and Siegler (1995). According to this model, four parameters contribute to strategic development and its efficiency:

  • 1) the repertoire of strategies, which corresponds to the different strategies available to the individual for solving problems;
  • 2) the distribution of strategies, which corresponds to the frequency with which each strategy is used;
  • 3) the effectiveness of strategies, defined as the accuracy and speed of execution of the strategy;
  • 4) the selection of strategies, which refers to the flexible choice of strategies.

The development of strategic skills would depend on the development of the four parameters thus defined. Furthermore, according to Lemaire and Siegler (1995), strategic flexibility depends on the effectiveness of each individual’s strategic skills in selecting the fastest strategy leading to the solution of the problem.

In line with Siegler’s model, Verschaffel’s work distinguishes between the notions of flexibility and adaptability, which may have different meanings depending on the authors. As noted by the author, for some researchers in the field, these notions are synonymous, while for others, these notions refer to two distinct competencies (e.g. Heinze et al. 2009). Thus, Verschaffel defines these notions as follows: strategic flexibility is the ability to choose flexibly between several strategies available in memory, but without necessarily selecting the most suitable one for the mathematical situation, whereas strategic adaptability also covers the ability to adopt the strategy most suitable for the problem.

More precisely, Verschaffel et al. (2009) introduce the notion of adaptability:

By an adaptive choice of a strategy, we mean the conscious or unconscious selection and use of the most appropriate solution strategy on a given mathematical item or problem, for a given individual, in a given sociocultural context (p. 343).

In addition, in this field of research, much work has investigated the effectiveness of different formats of external representations (e.g. graphs, diagrams, tables, etc.) that are provided to students to help them conceptualize and solve problems. In this case, representational flexibility refers to the ease with which the student uses multiple external representations and is able to switch from one to another to solve the problem. Acevedo Nistal et al. (2009) state that they use the term representational flexibility in the following way:

...We will use this term to speak about students’ disposition to make appropriate representational choices, taking into account the task, student and context characteristics that come into play in the resolution of the mathematical task at hand... (p. 629).

In this perspective, flexibility is defined as the student’s ability to switch between several external representations and select the most appropriate representation for solving the task. According to these authors, three factors influence this representational flexibility. The first refers to the characteristics of the task. The good fit between the external representations provided to solve the problem and the solution will facilitate the use and selection of the most appropriate external representation. The second factor concerns the characteristics of the student. Their conceptual and procedural knowledge of the conventional external representations provided in class, their knowledge of the problem-solving domain and their preferences and habits for using an external representation will determine the ease of flexible choice between several external representations. Finally, the context in which tasks are presented determines representational flexibility. Thus, devices that provide active support in the selection of external representations or that stimulate the comparison and evaluation of external representations play a significant role in students’ ability to use and choose the most relevant representation.

5.2.3.2. Research conducted in psychology

In psychology, research on the expression of flexibility in problem solving is still quite marginal. Indeed, in developmental psychology or neuropsychology, cognitive flexibility is usually assessed in the well-known switching task paradigm (see, for instance, Chapter 1, section 1.3.1.1).

In the cognitive approach of problem solving in which our work sits, we define cognitive flexibility to the individual’s ability to change mental representations of the situation, and not, as described previously, to the ability to switch from one external representation format to another. This nuance is important to emphasize, because often misunderstandings between researchers from different disciplinary fields are due to the semantics underlying the concepts used.

This approach is inherited from the pioneering work of Gestalt theory. Indeed, a classic example of mental rigidity, the corollary of flexibility, is what was described by the phenomenon of functional fixedness reported at the beginning of the 20th century by the gestaltists (Duncker 1945). Duncker reports that adult participants show great difficulty in spontaneously conceiving objects in an unconventional function and in using them in innovative ways to creatively solve problems. In some situations, knowledge about the conventional uses of objects prevents the discovery of creative solutions. The development of knowledge about the conventional use of objects due to the daily experiences of using familiar objects explains a result that may seem paradoxical at first sight: the development with age of a certain rigidity in the discovery of creative solutions.

In a study with children aged 5, 6, and 7, German and Defeyter (2000) only reported functional fixedness effects from age 6 onward, with 5-year-olds being much more creative than their elders. In this study, the authors created an adapted version of Duncker’s (1945) candle problem, the “Bobo the Bear’s House” problem, in which the child is asked to help a small teddy bear reach a toy sitting out of reach on a shelf. The children in the experiment were randomly assigned to two experimental conditions. In the first condition, a box filled with various objects constitutes a pre-use condition supposed to highlight the usual function of the box as a container. In the other condition, the objects are distributed around the box (condition without pre-use). The solution consists of using the box in an unusual function which serves as a stepping stone to grasp the toy. In the condition of no pre-use of the box, the 6- and 7-year-olds discovered the solution more quickly than their younger peers, while in the condition with pre-use of the box, the latter discovered the solution much more quickly than their elders. This result replicates the observations reported by gestaltists on the deleterious effects of past experiences and knowledge on the creative solutions discovery.

However, links between the development with age of cognitive flexibility in both linguistic and non-linguistic domains (see, for example, Deák (2003)) and the development of a certain rigidity that appears in the course of life experience remain to be understood and studied in more detail. The effect of schooling and the development of a certain conformism during adolescence are often evoked to explain this rigidity of behavior. However, it can be assumed that a principle of cognitive economy would be at the origin of such behaviors. In fact, it appears that behaviors that are certainly rigid are also effective most of the time in everyday life situations. Indeed, choosing whether or not to change one’s ways of doing and thinking can be cognitively costly. Nevertheless, this observation of rigid thinking linked to the school and cultural environment encourages future research on the development of educational programs stimulating cognitive flexibility to counteract this behavioral rigidity.

Concerning the distinction we make between strategic flexibility and representational flexibility that we defined as a change of viewpoint, we have been able to show that changing strategy or procedure is not sufficient to find the solution to a new problem that shares the same surface cues as some previously solved problems. This was empirically shown in an experiment with adults who were asked to solve Luchins’ jars problems (Clément 2006). In the terms used in transfer studies, the particularity of Luchins’ jars problems is that they share the same superficial indices (the number of available jars), but that the procedure to achieve the goal differs. In the first problems, the solution consists of using the three available jars, while in the following problems, it is possible to use two or three jars, and in the last problems, only two jars can be used among the three available.

The results show that, in the case of a solution with two jars, and even though the transfer strategy is simpler than with three jars, when some participants reach an impasse, that is, when the solution learned with three jars does not work, they change their strategy by testing new combinations, while continuing to use three jars without discovering the two jars solution. For these participants, a change in strategy or procedure may denote some form of strategic flexibility, but does not lead to solution discovery. Indeed, these participants do not show representational flexibility. They do not engage in processing the relevant properties to be considered, that is, the number of jars to be used. Thus, we defined the notion of representational, or conceptual, flexibility (see Chapter 4, section 4.3.2) as the ability to change our perspective, to re-represent the problem and the goal independently of the procedure; in other words, to engage in recoding and recategorizing the problem on the basis of its solution principle (Clément 2009).

5.3. Flexibility, creativity and academic performance

Interest in studying the flexible and creative processes at work in learning and education is not new, although since the 2000s there has been an exponential growth in work in this area (Craft 2005; Smith and Smith 2010; Huang et al. 2019; Hernández-Torrano and Ibrayeva 2020).

The first influential works on this question can be dated to the middle of the 20th century, with reference, on the one hand, to those of Gestalt theory, which we have already discussed (see section 5.2.3), and, on the other hand, to those of Guilford (1950), which marked the field of creativity studies. The latter defined creativity as a component of learning and stated that a complete theory of learning could not exclude both insight (i.e. the sudden discovery of the solution described by the gestaltists) and creative production.

In these terms, Guilford (1950) writes:

Learning theorists have had considerable difficulty with the behavior known as insight, to which creative behavior shows much apparent relationship. It is proper to say that a creative act is an instance of learning, for it represents a change in behavior that is due to stimulation and/or response. A comprehensive learning theory must take into account both insight and creative activity (pp. 445–446).

Although they do not explicitly refer to Guilford’s pioneering work, it is worth noting that today educational policies in a growing number of countries emphasize the importance of developing and promoting students’ creative potential throughout the school curriculum (Craft 2007; Beghetto 2010; Hui and Lau 2010; Lin 2011; Mourgues et al. 2014). However, there is still much work to be done in this area, as evidenced by recent research findings that highlight the gap between the creative potential that students express in extracurricular activities and the lack of creativity they may express in school activities (Runco et al. 2016; Runco 2017).

In the following section, before presenting the empirical results on the contribution of flexibility and creativity to academic performance, it seems appropriate to discuss the links that have been established in the scientific literature between these two notions.

5.3.1. Flexibility and creativity: what links?

Since the first works of Guilford (1950, 1967), the flexible character of cognition is considered as one of the components of creativity; it has been shown that its expression varies greatly depending on the individual (Guilford 1967; Torrance 1968; Runco 1995). According to Guilford, creative thinking is founded on two broad categories of abilities: the first includes the divergent production abilities, and the second refers to what the author called the transformation abilities.

Divergent thinking is the production of a wide variety of ideas on a situation, event, object, etc.

According to Guilford, it involves:

  • 1) fluency, which is the number of different ideas produced;
  • 2) flexibility, defined as the production of ideas from different fields of knowledge;
  • 3) an ability to develop new ideas.

Transformation abilities refer to the ability to produce something new on the basis of the known. In more recent terms, these transformation abilities correspond to the processes implied in reasoning by analogy that we presented earlier (see section 5.2.2). More precisely, they “pertain to revising what one experiences or knows, thereby producing new forms and patterns” (Guilford, 1967, p. 8). In these transformation abilities, flexibility would be predominant.

At first sight, flexibility and creativity seem to cover the same processes. However, we can distinguish two forms of flexibility, one of which is more specifically assessed in divergent or creative thinking: the spontaneous flexibility.

Neuropsychological research with brain-damaged patients has shown that cognitive flexibility is not a unitary component, but can take two forms: an adaptive form, which consists of adapting to changes imposed by the environment, and a spontaneous form, which is the ability to consider the same object or concept in a new light. In other words, adaptative or reactive flexibility is expressed when the environment changes and the constraints of the task require a change of response for an adapted behavior. Spontaneous flexibility occurs when the subject presents a variety of responses in a stable environment that does not necessarily require change.

The measures of reactive flexibility are designed in classical rule-switching tasks, a detailed description of which is given in Chapter 1 (see section 1.3.1.1). The principle of these tasks is to classify objects according to a first criterion, and then to classify the same objects according to a second criterion (see Grant and Berg (1948) for the original Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; see Frye et al. (1995) for the Dimensional Change Card Sort test adapted for young children).

The most commonly used tasks to assess spontaneous flexibility are the divergent thinking tests. These tests require generating creative responses in so-called “open-ended” problems for which there is no single answer, but several possibilities. One of these tests is the Alternative Uses Test, created by Guilford (1967). This test asks you to imagine as many novel uses as possible for a familiar object, such as a brick, an umbrella or a sheet of paper. In this divergent thinking test, three principal measures are used:

  • – the fluency, that is, the number of different uses produced;
  • – the originality, by comparing the suggested uses with frequently suggested uses;
  • – the flexibility, which corresponds to the number of different categories (domains) into which the proposed uses can be grouped.

As can be noted, the divergent thinking tests are very similar to the problems of functional fixedness imagined by the gestaltists. In both cases, it is a question of imagining new uses for familiar objects, the difference being that in the problems of functional fixedness, it is not just a question of evoking and producing new uses as in the divergent thinking, but of finding a solution to a problem by inhibiting the primary function of an object to achieve a goal, for example, using a box as a step rather than as a container. These problem-solving situations are thus highly interesting, because they are both closer to everyday life situations, finalized by a goal to be reached and involving the implementation of flexibility through action to attain the goal.

5.3.2. Cognitive flexibility and academic performance

Whether it be scientific research in education, psychology or didactics, a broad consensus is emerging on the role played by flexibility in learning in general, and in school learning in particular. It should be noted, however, that studies on the links between academic performance and cognitive flexibility are essentially carried out in a conception where cognitive flexibility is defined as an executive process of attentional shifting from one stimulus to another, or from one task to another. For example, research from this perspective shows that executive processes – including working memory, inhibition and flexibility – significantly contribute to differences in students’ academic performance (Bull and Scerif 2001; Andersson 2008; Best et al. 2009; Kercood et al. 2017).

Specifically regarding mathematical problem solving, a live issue within the research community is whether cognitive flexibility is a factor that predicts students’ future academic performance in mathematics. Indeed, in the academic domain, it is commonly accepted that mathematics is one of the subjects that requires the ability to flexibly grasp different aspects of tasks and switch flexibly from one strategy to another depending on the demands of the task (see, for example, Bull et al. (2001, 2008); Blair et al. (2008); Heinze et al. (2009); Agostino et al. (2010); Clark et al. (2010); Yeniad et al. (2013)). In a recent study that aimed to determine whether cognitive flexibility predicts students’ performance on end-of-year exam terminal tests, Hästö et al. (2019) challenged Finnish first graders (11th grade) to solve linear equations of the type:

Image

and to find innovative solutions with regard to the classical procedure taught.

Indeed, for these problems, the method taught in the Finnish curriculum consists of four steps:

  • 1) calculating the terms in the brackets using distribution;
  • 2) combining the terms;
  • 3) moving the unknowns x to the right and the other terms to the left;
  • 4) dividing by the coefficient.

This method leads to the following calculations in four steps:

Image

A more innovative method in terms of calculation steps and the difficulty of carrying out the different steps consists of three solution steps and leads to the calculation:

Image

In their experiment, the authors measured flexibility by the number of alternative solutions proposed by the students. The results show that the flexibility measured in solving equations is related to students’ performance on end-of-year tests in mathematics, chemistry and native language. However, when controlled for accuracy of the proposed solutions, only the students’ level of mathematics is related to flexibility.

Although, in general, the links between flexibility and academic performance are reported in the literature, some works do not highlight links between these processes and academic performance (Espy et al. 2004; Monette et al. 2011). As noted by Yeniad et al. (2013), the discordant results reported in the literature on the links between flexibility and academic performance are likely related to:

  • 1) the heterogeneity of the tasks used to assess flexibility;
  • 2) the nature of the measures to assess performance (response time or accuracy of answers);
  • 3) the characteristics of the participants in terms of age, gender and socioeconomic background of the children/students assessed;
  • 4) the statistical control of the impact of intelligence on academic performance.

However, recent meta-analyses conducted by the authors on the relationship between shifting abilities – that is, reactive flexibility and performance in math problem solving and reading – qualify this finding. Analyzing 18 studies on the relationship between shifting and math skills and 16 studies on the relationship between shifting and reading skills – involving a total of 4,596 subjects – the authors report that the highest levels of performance on shifting tests are also associated with higher levels of performance on math tests. Similarly, in reading, the highest shifting scores are associated with the highest performance in reading tests. One possible interpretation of these links between reactive flexibility and academic performance is that the flexibility measured in the research presented above is a skill whose development is highly dependent on culturally transmitted knowledge and experiences.

Results recently reported by Legare et al. (2018) support this interpretation. Seeking to test whether cognitive flexibility is a general and coherent cognitive capacity, or rather is determined by task-and knowledge-specific factors, the authors conducted an experiment with children of American and South African descent aged 3 to 5 years. They proposed two tasks supposed to imply the cognitive flexibility to the children: a rule-switching task and a flexible inference of the meaning of new words task (Deák and Whiseheart 2015). The results show that in the flexible inference of new word meanings task, American and South African children show similar performances. However, in the rule-switching task, only American kindergarten children show an increase in performance between 3 and 5 years. The authors conclude that the development of flexibility over the course of age is a general process, but depends on and is expressed differently according to culturally transmitted and valued knowledge and skills. According to the authors, American children’s experience with preschool activities that involve participating in arbitrary rule-based exercises and interacting with and manipulating symbolic material would explain their improved performance on rule-changing tasks over the course of age.

5.3.3. Creativity and academic performance

In the field of education, over the past two decades there has been a renewed interest in the study of creativity (Craft 2005; Smith and Smith 2010; Huang et al. 2019). The increase in the number of scientific publications attests to this phenomenon.

As Hernández-Torrano and Ibrayeva (2020) point out, this is certainly due to the increase in empirical findings that highlight the involvement of creativity in cognitive development, and more generally, that describe its benefits in various activities of social, emotional or professional life (Sternberg 2002; Freund and Holling 2008; Hansenne and Legrand 2012; Gajda et al. 2017a, 2017b; Fanchini et al. 2019). This growing interest in the role played by creativity in different spheres of human activity is based on the belief that creativity is crucial in an increasingly complex, changing and uncertain world. In this sense, hopes rest on the role that education can play in the development of this skill (Scott et al. 2004; Davies et al. 2013; Chan and Yuen 2014).

Empirical research with young children shows the importance of stimulating creative thinking during the early years of schooling for the future development of cross-curricular and transferable skills in different academic, professional and personal domains (Craft 2011). The development of pedagogies focused on creative learning throughout the school and university curriculum is thus advocated in several European (Cachia et al. 2010) and American (Adams 2005) educational policies. Thus, many creativity training programs have been designed and differ in the cognitive processes stimulated, the training techniques, the media used for training or the type of exercises proposed.

In a meta-analysis of 156 training programs (Scott et al. 2004), training programs based on cognitive processes involved in the solving and formulation of problems or conceptual production and combinations were found to play an important and beneficial role in creative development (Ward and Kolomyts 2010). In the same sense, studies conducted with young children on the effect of programs based on reasoning by analogy show the benefits of this type of training on the cognitive development of children aged 5–7 years (Antonietti 2000) and its usefulness in many academic areas (Castillo 1998). The link between creativity and academic performance is not a new issue in the psychology of creativity (Guilford 1950; Wallach and Kogan 1965). Nevertheless, empirical research conducted on this topic presents ambiguous and even contradictory results.

In a recent meta-analysis of 120 research studies conducted on the relationship between creativity and academic performance since the 1960s, Gajda et al. (2017b) report a positive, albeit modest, relationship between creativity and academic performance. However, this positive relationship is highly dependent on the schools where the research is conducted. For example, in a study of 589 students from 34 Polish high schools, Gralewski and Karwowski (2012) show that the direction of correlations between creativity scores measured in a creativity test and academic performance depends on the schools where the research was conducted: in some schools, the correlations are strong and positive, in others they are negative and in others no correlations were found.

Moreover, the links between academic performance, intelligence and creativity are even more surprising. In schools where creativity is highly valued in academic achievement, the links with intelligence are weak. In contrast, in the case where intelligence is considered the key factor in academic success, the weight of creativity is relatively low. Gralewski and Karwowski interpret these results as the consequence of the different profiles of institutions and teachers with regard to the values and pedagogical practices which are promoted. Indeed, the results suggest that the links between creativity and academic performance are present in classrooms where teachers support and encourage their students’ creativity. Conversely, these links are negative, or at best do not exist, in classrooms where teachers have a more analytical and traditional practices (Beghetto 2006).

To better understand the factors that mediate the expression of creativity at school, Gajda et al. (2017a) studied the dynamics of creative learning in 204 children from 10 primary school classes. The authors compared teacher– student relationships during classroom discussions in three ad hoc constituted groups on the basis of correlational analysis between creativity, measured in a standardized test (Jellen and Urban 1986; Urban and Jellen 1996), and students’ academic performance. The students with the best creativity scores and the best academic performance (positive correlation) form the first group, the students with the best creativity scores and the worst academic performance (negative correlation) form the second group. The third group consists of students for whom no correlation is found between creativity and academic performance (no correlation). The analyses conducted show several things. First, in the first group, during discussions, teachers provide their students with more pedagogical and emotional support than the teachers in the other two groups. In addition, the teacher’s appreciation of creativity in the classroom is associated with active engagement of students in learning, as well as more speaking up and expressing ideas. Furthermore, in classrooms where creativity and academic performance are positively related, there are many quality interactions between the teacher and the students, leaving room for the exchange of ideas, whereas in classrooms where creativity and academic performance are negatively related, the teacher’s interventions are more directive, less numerous and shorter. Finally, in classrooms where there is no correlation between creativity and academic performance, the questions and ideas generated by students are not taken up by the teacher, leaving no room for exploration, development and deepening of students’ ideas.

These results underline the importance of training and enhancing the creative potential of students in school programs. Educational programs that focus on stimulating flexibility and creativity as the basis for learning can help to achieve this goal. Learning in a flexible way to combine by analogy (Sander 2000) and to transform existing knowledge and individual experiences is promising for the emergence of new, original and situationally adapted conceptual and/or linguistic productions.

To summarize, the work presented in this section shows that cognitive flexibility, creativity and academic performance have a complex relationship that depends to a large extent on the pedagogical programs instituted at school, as well as on the pedagogical practices of teachers and the values accorded to creativity in academic success. As we have just seen, flexibility, a component of creativity, promotes ideation processes and transformation skills. In this sense, it allows us to reinterpret and reorganize the known in order to create the new.

5.4. Conclusion

Whether viewed as the ability of students to switch from one resolution strategy to another and choose the one best suited to the problem (Lemaire and Siegler 1995; Kilpatrick et al. 2001; Baroody and Dowker 2003; Verschaffel et al. 2007; Torbeyns et al. 2018), or as the ability to change perspectives on the situation, that is, to re-represent the situation and the means to the goal by engaging in semantic recoding (Clément 2009; Gamo et al. 2010; Gros et al. 2020; Gvozdic and Sander 2020), the body of research in the field of problem solving shows the importance of cognitive flexibility in the discovery of creative and adapted solutions. It has also been shown that flexibility and creativity are intimately linked, flexibility being one of the components necessary for the expression of creative thinking. Empirically demonstrated, the crucial role of flexibility in learning raises the question of the challenge to schools to promote a flexible approach to knowledge acquisition.

In conclusion, the challenges of the research conducted on the stimulation of flexibility and creative potential in students are both fundamental and societal. They are fundamental issues through their contribution to the development of knowledge on the cognitive and psychological processes involved in creative thinking. They are also societal and educational issues for the “school of tomorrow” confronted with a world where access to information and innovation are undergoing profound changes. Indeed, in recent years there has been a growing number of countries where educational policies emphasize the importance of developing students’ creative potential throughout the school curriculum (Craft 2007; Beghetto 2010; Hui and Lau 2010; Lin 2011; Mourgues et al. 2014; Hernandez and Ibrayeva 2020). For example, in an article devoted to innovative educational approaches conducted in France, Rey and Feyfant (2012) point out:

In France, innovation is often associated with the idea of experimentation and is largely aimed at local adjustments designed to improve student success and personalize paths [...] without necessarily having a direct impact on teaching content. Elsewhere, innovation may even involve the development of a school-wide curriculum (p. 2).

In our opinion, it seems necessary and indispensable to develop research whose objective is to include an educational approach focused on the stimulation and valorization of flexibility and creative thinking in French school programs. Furthermore, it is crucial that this educational approach is not limited to artistic activities alone, but is transversal and transferable to the different fields of learning, throughout the school curriculum.

5.5. References

Acevedo Nistal, A., van Dooren, W., Clarebout, G., Elen, J., Verschaffel, L. (2009). Conceptualising, investigating and stimulating representational flexibility in mathematical problem solving and learning: A critical review. ZDM Mathematics Education, 41, 627–636.

Acevedo Nistal, A., van Dooren, W., Verschaffel, L. (2012). Flexibility in problem solving: Analysis and improvement. In Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning, Seel, N.M. (ed.). Springer, Boston, MA.

Adams, K. (2005). The sources of innovation and creativity. Paper, National Center on Education and the Economy for the New Commission on the Skills of the American Workforce, Washington.

Agostino, A., Johnson, J., Pascual-Leone, J. (2010). Executive functions underlying multiplicative reasoning: Problem type matters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105(4), 286–305.

Anderson, U. (2008). Working memory as a predictor of written arithmetical skills in children: The importance of central executive functions. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 181–203.

Antonietti, A. (2000). Enhancing creative analogies in primary school children. North American Journal of Psychology, 2, 75–84.

Baroody, A.J. and Dowker, A. (2003). The Development of Arithmetic Concepts and Skills: Constructing Adaptive Expertise. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ.

Bassok, M., Wu, L.L., Olseth, K.L. (1995). Judging a book by its cover: Interpretative effects of content on problem-solving transfer. Memory & Cognition, 23, 354–367.

Bassok, M., Chase, V.M., Martin, S.A. (1998). Adding apples and oranges: Alignment of semantic and formal knowledge. Cognitive Psychology, 35, 99–134.

Bassok, M., Pedigo, S.F., Oskarsson, A.T. (2008). Priming addition facts with semantic relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 343–352.

Beghetto, R.A. (2006). Creative self-efficacy: Correlates in middle and secondary students. Creativity Research Journal, 18(4), 447–457.

Beghetto, R.A. (2010). Creativity in the classroom. In The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity, Kaufman, J.C., Sternberg, R.J. (eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

Best, J.R., Miller, P.H., Jones, L.L. (2009). Executive functions after age 5: Changes and correlates. Developmental Review, 29(3), 180–200.

Blair, C., Knipe, H., Gamson, D. (2008). Is there a role for executive functions in the development of mathematics ability? Mind Brain and Education, 2(2), 80–89.

Brown, A.L. and Campione, J.C. (1984). Three faces of transfer: Implications for early competence, individual differences and instruction. In Advances in Developmental Psychology, Volume 3, Lamb, M., Brown, A., Rogoff, B. (eds). Erlbaum, Hillsdale.

Bull, R. and Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematical ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19, 273–293.

Bull, R., Espy, K.A., Wiebe, S.A. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33(3), 205–228.

Cachia, R., Ferrari, A., Ala-Mutka, K. (2010). Creative learning and innovative teaching: Final report on the study on creativity and innovation in education in the EU member states. Report, Office des publications officielles des Communautés européennes (OPOCE), Luxembourg.

Castillo, L. (1998). The effect of analogy instruction on young children’s metaphor comprehension. Roeper Review, 21, 27–31.

Catrambone, R. and Holyoak, K.J. (1989). Overcoming contextual limitations on problem-solving transfer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6), 1147–1156.

Chan, S. and Yuen, M. (2014). Personal and environmental factors affecting teachers’ creativity-fostering practices in Hong Kong. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 12, 69–77.

Chen, Z. and Daehler, M.W. (1989). Positive and negative transfer in analogical problem solving by 6-year-old children. Cognitive Development, 4, 327–344.

Clark, C.A.C., Pritchard, V.E., Woodward, L.J. (2010). Preschool executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics achievement. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1176–1191.

Clément, E. (2006). Approche de la flexibilité cognitive dans la problématique de la résolution de problème. L’Année psychologique, 106, 415–434.

Clément, E. (2009). La résolution de problème. À la découverte de la flexibilité cognitive. Armand Colin, Paris.

Clément, E. (forthcoming). Flexibilité cognitive et logique de découverte. In Les logiques de la pensée, Sander, E. (ed.). Odile Jacob, Paris.

Clément, E. and Richard, J.F. (1997). Knowledge of domain effects in problem representation: The case of Tower of Hanoi isomorphs. Thinking and Reasoning, 3, 133–157.

Craft, A. (2005). Creativity in Schools: Tensions and Dilemmas. Routledge, London.

Craft, A. (2007). Possibility thinking in the early years and primary classroom. In Creativity: A Handbook for Teachers, Tan, A.G. (ed.). World Scientific, Singapore.

Craft, A. (2011). Creativity and early years settings. In Developing Reflective Practice in the Early Years, Paige-Smith, A., Craft, A. (eds). Open University Press, Buckingham.

Davies, D., Jindal-Snape, D., Collier, C., Digby, R., Hay, P., Howe, A. (2013). Creative learning environments in education: A systematic literature review. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 8, 80–91.

Deák, G.O. (2003). The development of cognitive flexibility and language abilities. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 31, 273–328.

Deák, G.O. and Wiseheart, M. (2015). Cognitive flexibility in young children: General or task-specific capacity? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 138, 31–53.

Duncker, K.N. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58(5), 270.

Elia, I., van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M., Kolovou, A. (2009). Exploring strategy use and strategy flexibility in non-routine problem solving by primary school high achievers in mathematics. ZDM Mathematics Education, 41, 605–618.

Espy, K.A., McDiarmid, M.M., Cwik, M.F., Stalets, M.M., Hamby, A., Senn, T.E. (2004). The contribution of executive functions to emergent mathematic skills in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26(1), 465–486.

Fanchini, A., Jongbloed, J., Dirani, A. (2019). Examining the well-being and creativity of school children in France. Cambridge Journal of Education, 49(4), 391–416.

Freund, P.A. and Holling, H. (2008). Creativity in the classroom: A multilevel analysis investigating the impact of creativity and reasoning on GPA. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 309–318.

Frye, D., Zelazo, P.D., Palfai, T. (1995). Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive Development, 10, 483–527.

Gajda, A., Beghetto, R.A., Karwowski, M. (2017a). Exploring creative learning in the classroom: A multi-method approach. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 24, 250–267.

Gajda, A., Karwowski, M., Beghetto, R.A. (2017b). Creativity and academic achievement: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(2), 269.

Gamo, S., Sander, E., Richard, J.-F. (2010). Transfer of strategy use by semantic recoding in arithmetic problem solving. Learning and Instruction, 20(5), 400–410.

German, T.P. and Defeyter, M. (2000). Immunity to “functional fixedness” in young children. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7, 707–712.

Gralewski, J. and Karwowski, M. (2012). Creativity and school grades: A case from Poland. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 7, 198–208.

Grant, D.A. and Berg, E. (1948). A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in Weigl-type card-sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 404–411.

Gros, H., Thibaut, J.P., Sander, E. (2020). Semantic congruence in arithmetic: A new model for word problem solving. Educational Psychologist, 55, 1532–6985.

Guilford, J.P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444–454.

Guilford, J.P. (1967). Creativity: Yesterday, today, and tomorrow. Journal of Creative Behavior, 1, 3–14.

Gvozdic, K. and Sander, E. (2020). Learning to be an opportunistic word problem solver: Going beyond informal solving strategies. ZDM, 23(141), 1–13.

Hansenne, M. and Legrand, J. (2012). Creativity, emotional intelligence, and school performance in children. International Journal of Educational Research, 53, 264–268.

Hästö, P., Palkki, R., Tuomela, D., Star, J.R. (2019). Relationship between mathematical flexibility and success in national examinations. European Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 7, 1–13.

Heinze, A., Star, J.R., Verschaffel, L. (2009). Flexible and adaptive use of strategies and representations in mathematics education. ZDM Mathematics Education, 41, 535–540.

Hernández-Torrano, D. and Ibrayeva, L. (2020). Creativity and education: A bibliometric mapping of the research literature (1975–2019). Thinking Skills and Creativity [Online]. Avaliable at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2019.100625.

Hofstadter, D. and Sander, E. (2013). L’analogie. Coeur de la pensée. Odile Jacob, Paris.

Holyoak, K.J. (2005). Analogy. In The Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning, Holyoak, K.J., Morrison, R.G. (eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

Huang, C., Yang, C., Wang, S., Wu, W., Su, J., Liang, C. (2019). Evolution of topics in education research: A systematic review using bibliometric analysis. Educational Review, 72, 1–17.

Hui, A.N. and Lau, S. (2010). Formulation of policy and strategy in developing creativity education in four Asian Chinese societies: A policy analysis. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 44(4), 215–235.

Jellen, H. and Urban, K.K. (1986). The TCT-DP (Test for Creative Thinking – Drawing Production): An instrument that can be applied to most age and ability groups. Creative Child and Adult Quarterly, 11, 138–155.

Kercood, S., Lineweaver, T.T., Frank, C.C., Fromm, E.D. (2017). Cognitive flexibility and its relationship to academic achievement and career choice of college students with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Postsecondary Education and Disability, 30(4), 327–342.

Kilpatrick, J., Swafford, J., Findell, B. (2001). Adding It Up: Helping Children Learn Mathematics. National Academy Press, Washington.

Kotovsky, K. and Fallside, D. (1989). Representation and transfer in problem solving. In Complex Information Processing. What has Simon Brought? 21st Symposium of the Carnegie Mellon Institute, Kotovsky, K. (ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale.

Krems, J.F. (1995). Cognitive flexibility and complex problem solving. In Complex Problem Solving: The European Perspective, Frensch, P.A., Funke, J. (eds). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale.

Legare, C.H., Dale, M.T., Kim, S.Y., Deák, G.O. (2018). Cultural variation in cognitive flexibility reveals diversity in the development of executive functions. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 16326.

Lemaire, P. and Siegler, R.S. (1995). Four aspects of strategic change: Contributions to children’s learning of multiplication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 83–97.

Lin, Y.S. (2011). Fostering creativity through education: A conceptual framework of creative pedagogy. Creative Education, 2(3), 149–155.

Luchins, A.S. (1939). The Einstellung effect in learning by repetition. Unpublished PhD Thesis, New York University, New York.

Luchins, A.S. (1942). Mechanization in problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 54, 248.

Martin, S.A. and Bassok, M. (2005). Effects of semantic cues on mathematical modeling: Evidence from word-problem solving and equation construction tasks. Memory & Cognition, 33(3), 471–478.

Monette, S., Bigras, M., Guay, M.C. (2011). The role of the executive functions in school achievement at the end of Grade 1. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(2), 158–173.

Moss, J. and Case, T. (1999). Developing children’s understanding of the rational numbers: A new model and an experimental curriculum. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30, 122–148.

Mourgues, C., Barbot, B., Tan, M., Grigorenko, E.L. (2014). The interaction between culture and the development of creativity. In The Oxford Handbook of Human Development and Culture: An Interdisciplinary Perspective, Jensen, L.A. (ed.). Oxford Library of Psychology, Oxford.

Novick, L.R. (1988). Analogical transfer, problem similarity, and expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 510–520.

Raynal, L., Clément, E., Sander, E. (2020). Are superficially similar analogs better retrieved than superficially similar analogs? Acta Psychologica, 203, 102989.

Rey, O. and Feyfant, A. (2012). Vers une éducation plus innovante et créative. Dossier d’Actualité Veille et Analyses, 70, 1–20.

Runco, M.A. (1995). Insight for creativity, expression for impact. Creativity Research Journal, 8, 377–390.

Runco, M.A. (2017). Comments on where the creativity research has been and where it is going. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 51(4), 308–313.

Runco, M.A., Acar, S., Campbell, W.K., McCain, J., Gentile, B. (2016). Comparisons of the creative class and regional creativity with perceptions of community support and community barriers. Business Creativity and Creative Economy, 2, 24–32.

Sander, E. (2000). L’analogie, du naïf au créatif : analogie et catégorisation. L’Harmattan, Paris.

Scott, G., Leritz, L.E., Mumford, M.D. (2004). The effectiveness of creativity training: A quantitative review. Creativity Research Journal, 16(4), 361–388.

Smith, J.K. and Smith, L.F. (2010). Educational creativity. In The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity, Kaufman, J.C., Sternberg, R.J (eds). Cambridge University Press, New York.

Spencer, R.M. and Weisberg, R.W. (1986). Context-dependent effects on analogical transfer. Memory & Cognition, 14(5), 442–449.

Sternberg, R.J. (2002). Raising the achievement of all students: Teaching for successful intelligence. Educational Psychology Review, 14, 383–393.

Torbeyns, J., Peters, G., De Smedt, B., Ghesquière, P., Verschaffel, L. (2018). Subtraction by addition strategy use in children of varying mathematical achievement level: A choice/no-choice study. Journal of Numerical Cognition, 4(1), 215–234.

Torrance, E.P. (1968). A longitudinal examination of the fourth grade slump in creativity. Gifted Child Quarterly, 12(4), 195–199.

Urban, K. and Jellen, H. (1996). Test for Creative Thinking – Drawing Production. Swets Test Services, Frankfurt.

Verschaffel, L., Greer, B., De Corte, E. (2007). Whole number concepts and operations. In Second Handbook of Research in Mathematics Teaching and Learning, Lester, F.K. (ed.). Macmillan, New York.

Verschaffel, L., Luwel, K., Torbeyns, J., van Dooren, W. (2009). Conceptualizing, investigating, and enhancing adaptive expertise in elementary mathematics education. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 24(3), 335–359.

Vosniadou, S. and Ortony, A. (1989). Similarity and Analogical Reasoning. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

Wallach, M.A. and Kogan, N. (1965). Models of Thinking in Young Children. Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York.

Ward, T.B. and Kolomyts, Y. (2010). Cognition and creativity. In The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity, Kaufman, J.C., Sternberg, R.J. (eds). Cambridge University Press, New York.

Yeniad, N., Malda, M., Mesman, J., van Ijzendoorn, M.H., Pieper, S. (2013). Shifting ability predicts math and reading performance in children: A meta-analytical study. Learning and Individual Differences, (23), 1–9.

..................Content has been hidden....................

You can't read the all page of ebook, please click here login for view all page.
Reset
18.223.122.15