Chapter Six

Aging, neurocognitive reserve, and the healthy brain

Chih-Mao Huanga,b; Hsu-Wen Huangc,*    a Department of Biological Science and Technology, National Chiao Tung University, Hsinchu, Taiwan
b Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Institute of Linguistics, Academia Sinica, Taipei, Taiwan
c Department of Linguistics and Translation, City University of Hong Kong, Kowloon Tong, Hong Kong
* Corresponding author: email address: [email protected]

Abstract

Healthy older adults experience a general decrement in physical and cognitive abilities with advancing age. The severity of these behavioral and neurocognitive declines is highly variable within the aged population. The Neurocognitive Reserve Hypothesis has been proposed in the cognitive and clinical neuroscience of aging to suggest that mentally-stimulating activities and life-long experiences may provide reserve—a protective mechanism that increases the brain's capacity to cope with age-related pathology. This model of the neurocognitive reserve hypothesis has successfully provided a theoretical account for the disjunction between the degree of observed brain damage/pathology and the clinical manifestations of that damage, both structurally and functionally. This article briefly reviews the behavioral and neuroimaging evidence that neurocognitive reserve shapes age-related and individual differences in neurocognitive processes, patterns of neural activation, brain structures and neural networks. Moreover, existing theoretical frameworks proposed in the aging literature are introduced to complement the understanding of neurocognitive reserve in normal and pathological aging. Finally, we report preliminary functional and structural neuroimaging results to support the hypothesis that neurocognitive reserve is a neural resource that mitigates not only the effects of cognitive decline caused by neurological diseases and/or psychiatric disorders, but also those caused by the general aging process. We conclude that there is currently limited understanding of the mechanisms underlying neurocognitive reserve; however, the concept provides a dynamic view for understanding the nature of resilience and our ability to adapt as we age to cope with brain pathology and damage. Future studies may consider decoding the individualized factors potentially underpinning neurocognitive reserve's beneficial contribution to protecting against accelerated cognitive decline and to promoting psychological resilience with advanced aging.

Keywords

Cognitive aging; Neurocognitive reserve; Individual differences; fMRI; Geriatric depression

1 Introduction

In the last few decades, most highly developed nations in the world have experienced substantial increases in the proportion of elderly adults in the population due to declining fertility and increased life expectancies (Lutz, Sanderson, & Scherbov, 2008). The financial burdens of medical care affect all age ranges, but this distribution is heavily skewed toward those in the later years of their life. Thus, the phenomenon of global aging and longevity represents varied challenges, including social, economic, and medical issues. Older adults face the potential storm of physiological, neurocognitive, and psychological deterioration that can heavily impact their daily functioning related to attention, memory, reasoning, and comprehension skills. This is called cognitive decline. Among the issues associated with aging, much focus has been on severe physiological impairments such as cardiovascular disease, cancer, and diabetes, but less attention has been paid to dementia.

One of the subtypes of dementia, Alzheimer's disease (AD), accounts for 60–70% of cases and is thus the most common neurodegenerative disease that impacts cognitive impairments beyond the normal aging processes. Therefore, it is critical to think about dementia in terms of its cognitive and neuropsychological aspects. During the disease's progression, AD patients suffer from the progressive and chronic deterioration of multiple cognitive functions. The major clinical and psychiatric deteriorations in the early stage of AD usually start from memory, visuo-spatial and language deficits, loss of motivation, and problems of judgment, absolute reasoning, and computational ability (Burns & Iliffe, 2009). Patients with AD may then gradually develop changes of personality, loss of episodic and autobiographic memory, dysregulation of emotional responses, illusionary misidentifications, hallucinations, and delusional symptoms (Burns & Iliffe, 2009), leading up to a loss of core bodily functions. To date, the incidence of AD doubles every 5 years after the age of 65. According to Alzheimer's Disease International, more than 35 million people in the world are affected by AD, and this number is predicted to rise to 115 million people by 2050 (http://www.alz.co.uk/). The rising cost of senior health and medical care from noncommunicable and/or chronic diseases associated with pathological aging (e.g., dementia) has become a serious burden on social services, government structures, and the economy. In addition, the treatment of AD has become an unaffordable drain on medical resources and financial and social welfare in many countries. For instance, AD ranks as one of the 10 deadliest diseases in the United States (Alzheimer's Association, 2015). Pharmaceutical Research and Manufacturers of America, PhRMA (2013) reported that more than 5 million Americans are living with AD, which costs more than US$200 billion for medical treatment and healthcare services. Best estimations suggest that these costs will increase to $1.2 trillion by 2050.

Given the future paths of population aging and the lack of disease-modifying treatment for dementia at present, early prevention becomes paramount in trying to prevent subsequent disability. There is thus an urgent need for theoretical guidelines for “successful aging” to help elderly individuals achieve and maintain a healthy functioning state. Successful aging refers to (1) active maintenance of neurocognitive and physical functions; (2) conservation of social ties and productive activities sustained by engagement; (3) avoidance of neural diseases and disability (Rowe & Kahn, 1997). These goals have motivated researchers in cognitive, social, and clinical psychology, gerontology, and geriatric medicine to conduct several cross-sectional and longitudinal studies of cognitive aging in order to identify potential factors that protect against accelerated cognitive decline with advanced aging and the onset of dementia. The Neurocognitive Reserve Hypothesis has been proposed to suggest that educational and occupation exposure, leisure activities, and mentally stimulating activities may provide cognitive reserve, a protective mechanism that increases the brain's capacity to cope with pathology in the elderly, suggesting potential ways to maintain successful aging (Cabeza et al., 2018; Stern, 2002, 2007, 2009, 2012).

In this chapter, we briefly review age-related differences in behavior and cognition and consider evidence in particular suggesting that such behavioral changes may stem from functional and structural brain changes across a lifespan, as measured by advanced non-invasive neuroimaging techniques such as magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). We then focus on the hypothesis that a variety of mentally stimulating activities and lifelong experiences may provide a cognitive and brain reserve (i.e., a neurocognitive reserve), thereby increasing the brain's capacity to cope with age-related pathology and producing varied patterns of performance and skill among older adults. Following that, we discuss extant measures/indicators of neurocognitive reserve and then introduce two neural measures adapted to investigate the associations between neurocognitive reserve and individual differences in neural structure and function. Moreover, we will complement existing theoretical frameworks proposed in the cognitive neuroscience of aging literature (i.e., GOLDEN, CRUNCH, and STAC) to push forward an understanding of the neurocognitive reserve in both normal and pathological aging. We will close by describing some emerging neuroimaging findings to support the hypothesis that neurocognitive reserve indicates brain resources accumulated over a lifespan, which can provide more avenues for the brain to adapt to the cognitive decline caused by aging in a more general sense.

2 Cognitive and brain aging

There is a wealth of literature that documents age-related changes in fundamental cognitive processes across the lifespan, which include both decline and preservation in cognition and behavior (Park et al., 2002). Age-related declines are often reported as the dominant picture of cognitive aging, with slower speed of processing (Salthouse, 1996), reduced sensory processing (Baltes & Lindenberger, 1997), decreased working memory capacity (Park et al., 2002, 1996), diminished ability in attentional processing (Hasher & Zacks, 1988), deficient ability to perform task switching (Hasher & Zacks, 1988), and inefficiency for sentence processing (Federmeier & Kutas, 2005; Wlotko, Lee, & Federmeier, 2010) highlighted as some of the major characteristics. All of the behavioral measurements of cognitive functions described above show age-related declines for many literate older adults across several cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. In contrast, preserved cognition is evidenced by cross-sectional studies which show that verbal knowledge and world knowledge (Park et al., 2002), and implicit, procedural memory (Howard, Howard, Dennis, LaVine, & Valentino, 2008) remain relatively intact across the lifespan. In addition, the severity of declines in behavioral and neurocognitive abilities is highly variable within the aged population (Goh, An, & Resnick, 2012; Li, 2003), highlighting the importance of considering individual differences in aging studies.

2.1 Aging, individual variation, and cognition

Aging is a process that occurs in a unique manner with each individual. It is a multi-dimensional process in which the individual's functional and health status are influenced by a variety of genetic, environmental, and cultural factors (Goh & Huang, 2012; Li, 2003; Na, Huang, & Park, 2017). The most commonly reported age-related and individual variance in cognition is that executive function performance decreases with increasing age (Bopp & Verhaeghen, 2005). Several theoretical accounts of cognitive aging have been developed to explain age-related and individual differences in the domain of executive function. For example, Salthouse (1996) proposed that aging leads to reduced speed of processing, rendering it more difficult to maintain several items in short-term memory at a time. Craik and Byrd (1982) suggested that as one ages, the resources available to perform a given task decrease, resulting in less working memory capacity in older adults. In addition, it is also possible to link age-related decline in cognition to a reduced ability to maintain an appropriate attention focus and/or reduced inhibition of the processing of task-irrelevant information (Hasher & Zacks, 1988). Within these theories lies the notion that in a broad array of cognitive behaviors, age-related variance is mediated by task demands as well as individual variability (Park et al., 2002, 1996). Moreover, the inhomogeneous pace of aging among our cognitive capacities is further complicated by individual differences in genotypes, cultural backgrounds (Hedden et al., 2002; Li, 2003; Na et al., 2017; Park et al., 2002), life style (Bugg & Head, 2011; Nyberg, Lövdén, Riklund, Lindenberger, & Bäckman, 2012; Valenzuela & Sachdev, 2006), and language (Bialystok, Craik, & Freedman, 2007; Bialystok, Klein, Craik, & Viswanathan, 2004).

Given the prominent role of language in social interactions, there has been a growing literature examining individual variability in language processing across the human lifespan. Indeed, comprehending language is a complex cognitive process, requiring that words be rapidly decoded and concepts linked together to develop message-level meaning representation. Studies have shown that skilled readers vary in the extent to which they use language context predictively to activate words and their meanings (Federmeier, 2007) and the extent to which they allocate attention to integrate meanings across words in text (Stine-Morrow & Miller, 2009; Stine-Morrow, Miller, & Hertzog, 2006). In addition, readers also vary in their levels of cognitive engagement with text (Guthrie et al., 2004; Stine-Morrow, 2007). These variations have been related to individual differences in vocabulary and verbal fluency (Federmeier, Kutas, & Schul, 2010; Kutas & Federmeier, 2011), working memory capacity (Payne, Gao, Noh, Anderson, & Stine-Morrow, 2012), and age (Federmeier et al., 2010; Federmeier, Van Petten, Schwartz, & Kutas, 2003; Stine-Morrow, Milinder, Pullara, & Herman, 2001). The age-related declines in general cognitive functioning (speed, attention, inhibitory control, and memory) that have already been discussed necessarily also affect on-line language processing. For example, older adults have difficulty inhibiting lexical competitors (Sommers & Danielson, 1999), making word recognition more difficult. Event-related potentials (ERPs) studies have shown that older adults have reduced abilities to use sentence context information to facilitate word processing (Federmeier et al., 2003), and to predict upcoming semantic information (Federmeier, McLennan, De Ochoa, & Kutas, 2002). Moreover, older adults are less able to form an integrated conceptual representation during language comprehension (Huang, Meyer, & Federmeier, 2012). Taken together, age constitutes an essential variable of language change across the life span. However, there are other variables that may modulate age effects, among which we will discuss level of education and bilingualism in the later sections.

2.2 Structure of the aging brain

2.2.1 Shrinkage of the aging brain and its association with cognitive function

Understanding the atrophy of the aging brain is essential to understanding age-related and individual differences in neurocognitive functions across a lifespan. There is a wealth of neuroimaging evidence, including structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) across several cross-sectional and longitudinal studies, which demonstrates that biological aging is characterized by reductions in gray matter volume (Raz et al., 2005; Resnick, Pham, Kraut, Zonderman, & Davatzikos, 2003; Rodrigue & Raz, 2004), density (Good et al., 2001; Smith, Chebrolu, Wekstein, Schmitt, & Markesbery, 2007), and cortical thickness (Dickerson et al., 2009; Fjell et al., 2006, 2009; Salat et al., 2004), with the prefrontal, parietal and hippocampal regions showing greater shrinkage compared to the temporal and occipital regions, which show more minimal decreases. For example, Raz et al. (2005) reported volumetric measurements of a number of structural brain MRI images in lifespan samples, tracking both cross-sectional and longitudinal change over 5 years. They showed gradual volumetric changes with age, but also found that these changes are not equivalent across brain regions, with the greatest age-related shrinkage occurring in the prefrontal, hippocampal, caudate, and cerebellar regions. Relative stability was observed in the entorhinal cortex and the visual regions of the brain (Raz et al., 2005).

Given that the performance of neurocognitive functions may stem from changes in the brain's structural cytoarchitecture, many studies have embarked on associating age-related differences in regional brain volume with individuals' cognitive functioning measured by a variety of neuropsychological tests. Although structure-cognition associations are not easily replicated and appear to be sensitive to subtypes of neurocognitive assessment and to sample composition, there is also clear evidence that these age-related reductions in brain structure may index specific cognitive decline in older adults. For example, age-related volumetric shrinkage in prefrontal gray matter has been related to lower fluid intelligence (Fjell et al., 2006; Head, Rodrigue, Kennedy, & Raz, 2008), poorer inhibitory/motor control and working memory performance (Head, Kennedy, Rodrigue, & Raz, 2009; Tapp et al., 2004), but better verbal fluency (Elderkin-Thompson, Ballmaier, Hellemann, Pham, & Kumar, 2008). Moreover, reduced hippocampal volume has been reported to be associated with poorer episodic memory (Head et al., 2008; Rodrigue & Raz, 2004).

2.2.2 Age-related reduction in micro-structural neural fibers and its cognitive consequences

In addition to the shrinkage in gray matter, biological aging is also associated with a reduction in white matter volume and density (Raz et al., 2005; Resnick et al., 2003; Salat et al., 2009; Walhovd et al., 2011). More recently, studies using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), a non-invasive imaging technique, have suggested that white matter changes with aging involve a reduction in the integrity of white matter fibers that may be related to de-myelination along the axonal fibers and may result in less efficient conduction of neural signals and impaired transmission of information across the brain (Cox et al., 2016; Fan et al., 2019; Madden, Bennett, & Song, 2009; Sullivan & Pfefferbaum, 2006; Yang, Tsai, Liu, Huang, & Lin, 2016). The most consistent pattern of age-related difference in white matter fiber integrity is the greater reduction in the anterior regions of the corpus callosum, especially in the genu and body of the corpus callosum and pericallosal frontal white matter, relative to the posterior regions of the corpus callosum (Davis, Kragel, Madden, & Cabeza, 2011; Fan et al., 2019; Gordon et al., 2008; Gunning-Dixon & Raz, 2003; Sullivan & Pfefferbaum, 2006; Sullivan, Rohlfing, & Pfefferbaum, 2010), reflecting an anterior-to-posterior deterioration in white-matter diffusivity and anisotropy with age.

There is also converging evidence that age-related reductions in white matter microstructural integrity have direct consequences for cognitive performance (Bennett & Madden, 2014; Fan et al., 2019; Ritchie et al., 2015). With regard to processing speed, Kennedy and Raz (2009) found that slowed speed of cognitive processing was related to a reduced integrity in anterior white matter regions, suggesting that general slowing in older adults may stem from degraded neural transmission along the axonal fibers of the aging brain. Moreover, with regard to working memory, Charlton et al. (2006) first reported a significant correlation between working memory performance and white matter integrity, with Digit-Symbol sequencing performance negatively correlated with integrity in anterior, middle, and posterior portions of the aging brain. Zahr, Rohlfing, Pfefferbaum, and Sullivan (2009) further reported a link between working memory and the corpus callosum, with poor working memory in older adults relating to less integrity of the anterior portion of the corpus callosum. Additionally, Kennedy and Raz (2009) found that poorer working memory in older adults appeared to be associated with age-related reduction in white matter integrity in widespread networks of regions ranging from anterior (prefrontal, anterior corpus callosum) to posterior (temporal and occipital) regions of the brain. These DTI findings suggest that working memory performance depends on white matter integrity in a widespread network of the aging brain. Finally, with regard to the domain of cognitive control, increased task switching costs have been reported to be associated with reduced integrity of fronto-parietal white matter (Gold, Powell, Xuan, Jicha, & Smith, 2010), integrity of prefrontal (Gratton, Wee, Rykhlevskaia, Leaver, & Fabiani, 2009), anterior corpus callosum, superior parietal, and occipital white matter (Kennedy & Raz, 2009), as well as the genu of the corpus callosum (Madden et al., 2009). Also, higher Stroop interference costs were found to be associated with reduced anisotropy of the posterior white matter in older adults, particularly in parietal, splenium, and occipital regions of the aging brain (Kennedy & Raz, 2009). Moreover, Davis et al. (2009) used a fiber tracking technique on young and older adults, and reported that greater white matter integrity in anterior regions but not posterior regions of the aging brain was related to better executive functioning (Davis et al., 2009). In addition, our recent DTI findings demonstrated that increased micro-structural integrity in both anterior corpus callosum and right cingulum/angular fibers positively correlated with performance on a visuospatial task in older adults. The further mediation analysis revealed that the white matter integrity of the frontal inter-hemispheric fibers was a significant mediator of the age-and-visuospatial performance relation in older adults, but not in younger adults (Fan et al., 2019). These results suggest that, congruent with the findings in the working memory domain, performance of executive control in older adults appears to depend on white matter integrity in a widespread network of the aging brain.

2.3 Functional neuroimaging of cognitive aging

2.3.1 Greater and more distributed neural activity with age

Age-related patterns of change, including more activity and more distributed activity, especially in the fronto-parietal cortices, have been frequently reported across a variety of cognitive and motor domains, including language comprehension, working memory, cognitive control, and motor function (Cabeza et al., 2018; Grady, 2012; Park & Reuter-Lorenz, 2009). In functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) studies, healthy older participants exhibited either (1) greater bilateral activity than the more unilateral activity observed in their young counterparts, and/or (2) additional and more distributed activation in regions that are not activated in young adults (Cabeza, Anderson, Locantore, & McIntosh, 2002; Cabeza et al., 2004; Huang, Meyer, & Federmeier, 2012; Huang, Polk, Goh, & Park, 2012; Jimura & Braver, 2010). Therefore, the phenomena of greater and more distributed fronto-parietal activity in older adults has been identified as a general characteristic of age-related functional neural change (Cabeza et al., 2018, 2004; Park & Reuter-Lorenz, 2009).

Age-related greater activity in fronto-parietal regions is often interpreted as being compensatory and involved in the improvement or maintenance of behavioral performance in the face of age-related neurodegeneration (Cabeza et al., 2002; Davis, Dennis, Daselaar, Fleck, & Cabeza, 2008; Heuninckx, Wenderoth, & Swinnen, 2008; Huang, Meyer, & Federmeier, 2012; Huang, Polk, Goh, & Park, 2012; Vallesi, McIntosh, & Stuss, 2011). For example, Huang, Meyer, and Federmeier (2012) and Huang, Polk, Goh, and Park (2012) investigated the functional significance of posterior parietal cortex when young and older adults performed a physical-numerical interference paradigm (i.e., numerical Stroop paradigm) while undergoing fMRI. In this paradigm, stimuli were pairs of Arabic digits shown simultaneously in the middle of a computer screen. Two types of magnitude judgments were included. For the numerical judgment task, participants were asked to judge which digit was numerically larger while ignoring their physical sizes. For the physical size task, participants were required to decide which one was physically larger while ignoring the numerical magnitude of the digits. In congruent trials, the digit that was larger in numerical magnitude was also larger in physical size. In incongruent trials, the digit that was larger in numerical magnitude was smaller in physical size. We found that, in the Incongruent vs. Congruent conditions contrast, young adults engaged more right parietal cortex for the Magnitude task and activated more left parietal cortex for the Size task, whereas older adults showed bilateral parietal activation for both Size and Magnitude tasks. These findings suggested a task-specific neural recruitment in the parietal cortex, as the hemisphere that was engaged depended on the judgment required. Moreover, we demonstrated the age-related additional parietal activity (i.e., right parietal for the Size and left parietal for the Magnitude task in older adults) was associated with better performance. Finally, as in many previous studies, we showed that older adults also recruited their left prefrontal cortex during both tasks, and this common activation was also associated with better performance (Huang, Meyer, & Federmeier, 2012; Huang, Polk, Goh, & Park, 2012). These results provide evidence for task-specific compensatory recruitment in the parietal cortex as well as task-independent compensatory recruitment in the left prefrontal cortex during cognitive control in normal aging.

Several functional neuroimaging studies use similar designs to explore compensatory mechanisms of the aging brain, and also suggest that greater and more distributed neural activation in anterior brain areas (e.g., prefrontal cortex), in addition to being beneficial for behavioral performance, may serve a compensatory role for declining neural function in more posterior sites, including the medial temporal lobe (Cabeza et al., 2004; Gutchess et al., 2005; Park et al., 2003), and occipital visual areas (Cabeza et al., 2004; Davis et al., 2008; Goh, Suzuki, & Park, 2010). For example, given the observation of age-related cognitive declines in memory encoding and retrieval and volumetric shrinkage in the memory-related hippocampal area, Gutchess et al. (2005) conducted an fMRI study to investigate the functional relationship between prefrontal and medial temporal activation during an incidental memory encoding task when both young and older participants were scanned. The results showed that older adults had reduced activation compared to young adults in the left and right parahippocampus. More importantly, such reduced activation in the medial temporal lobe was significantly coupled with greater activation in the middle frontal cortex. Similarly, Goh et al. (2010) also demonstrated that older adults showed reduced activation in occipital visual areas during a face discrimination task. However, these decreased neural responses were associated with greater activation in the prefrontal cortex for older adults compared to their young counterparts. These findings are consistent with the Posterior Anterior Shift in Aging (PASA) hypothesis, which posits a neurocognitive compensatory role of prefrontal regions for age-related neural deterioration in posterior brain regions (Cabeza et al., 2004; Davis et al., 2008).

Overall, these findings suggest that the brain ages in a dynamic way, declining in some respects but maintaining the ability to engage adaptive neural functions even in advanced age.

2.3.2 Altered functional connectivity with age

There is converging evidence that biological aging is associated with significant changes in functional connectivity between different brain regions, resulting in less effective neural communication between different areas in the aging brain (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007; Damoiseaux et al., 2008; Duzel, Schutze, Yonelinas, & Heinze, 2011; Persson, Lustig, Nelson, & Reuter-Lorenz, 2007). Functional connectivity analysis involves correlating low-frequency neuronal signal in resting-state functional MRI data (data are acquired as participants simply lay still in the MRI scanner) between predefined regions-of-interest (ROIs). Several studies have suggested that, compared to younger counterparts, there is a posterior-to-anterior gradient of decline in functional connectivity in older adults. The findings suggest that age-related reductions in occipital activity and increases in prefrontal activity (i.e., PASA) may be associated with less effective functional communication between neural networks of the aging brain. Moreover, some studies measuring functional connectivity during the resting state among predefined ROIs have demonstrated that intrinsic brain connectivity in the aging brain is related to the performance of older individuals on a variety of cognitive tasks, including executive function and processing speed (Damoiseaux et al., 2008), associative memory (Wang et al., 2010), and working memory (Sambataro et al., 2010).

Thus far, we have reviewed findings from human neuroimaging studies showing age-related changes in brain volume and cortical thickness, micro-structural neural fibers, functional activation, and functional connectivity. However, interpreting age-related cognitive declines in the context of structural and functional changes in the aging brain at multiple levels of analysis is an ongoing challenge. Aging continues to be associated with cognitive decline and preservation in many individuals; what are the putative causal explanations for the middle-aged and/or older adults who exhibit substantial cognitive loss (and may develop dementia over the next few years) and for those who show intact cognitive abilities over the age of 80? Can middle-aged and older adults rely upon neural resources (i.e., neurocognitive reserve) to compensate for age-related declines in cognitive function and structural brain volume? In the next section, we will discuss whether and how the concept of neurocognitive reserve interacts with age-related and individual differences in cognition, brain structure, and neural function.

3 The neurocognitive reserve hypothesis

The Reserve Hypothesis has been proposed to suggest that a variety of mentally-stimulating activities may provide “reserve,” a protective mechanism that increases the brain's capacity to cope with age-related pathology, including mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia, resulting in greater individual variation across the human lifespan (Stern, 2002; Stern, Arenaza-Urquijo, et al., 2018; Stern, Gazes, Razlighi, Steffener, & Habeck, 2018). According to the reserve hypothesis, the pathologic conditions, brain damage, and neuropsychological function of geriatric patients with neurological diseases might be related to individual differences based on their life experiences, educational and occupational exposure, and leisure activities, which can reduce the risk of developing dementia and slow the rate of cognitive decline in late adulthood (Stern, 2009). Moreover, Stern (2009) noted that the concept of a reserve is relevant not just to patterns related to pathological aging (e.g., the onset of dementia) but also to the normal aging process, with the idea that the mechanisms of reserve may allow individuals to cope more effectively with a variety of age-related cognitive and brain changes.

3.1 Brain and cognitive reserve

The concept of reserve has been developed into two sub-concepts: brain reserve and cognitive reserve (Stern, 2009; Stern, Arenaza-Urquijo, et al., 2018; Stern, Gazes, et al., 2018). The brain reserve hypothesis (BR) suggests that morphological differences, such as brain volume, head circumference, synaptic count, or dendritic branching, account for individual variability in resistance to brain damage. In addition to medical postmortem examination and epidemiological research, advanced non-invasive neuroimaging techniques, including structural/anatomical MRI, provide an elegant approach for examining the neural implementation of variables related to brain reserves. The possible indices of brain reserve in the neurobiology of aging literature could be brain size or level of atrophy (Perneczky et al., 2010; Querbes et al., 2009), white matter integrity (Teipel et al., 2009), cerebral blood flow, glucose metabolism activity, and brain network connectivity during tasks and rest. Christensen, Anstey, Leach, and Mackinnon (2008) found that higher intelligence was associated with larger brain volumes; however, this relationship declined with advancing age (Christensen et al., 2008). Katzman et al. (1988) suggested that patients with larger brains can sustain more brain damage because a larger brain can provide sufficient neural substrate to support normal cognitive functions. Older people who have a larger brain volume have a lower risk of developing dementia (Katzman et al., 1988; Schofield, Logroscino, Andrews, Albert, & Stern, 1997; Stern, 2012). Evidence also suggests that a critical threshold exists: patients will not have any clinical expression of disability until reaching this threshold.

On the other hand, the cognitive reserve hypothesis (CR) refers to the efficiency, capacity, and flexibility of human cognitive processes that can contribute to individual differences in age-related susceptibility of cognitive abilities or day-to-day function to brain changes. Furthermore, CR suggests that individual differences developed through life experiences will provide a protective and beneficial mechanism to maintain brain networks and cognitive functions to cope with brain aging, brain damage, brain pathology, and/or insult. There is extensive epidemiological and experimental evidence that some life experiences, such as educational and occupational attainment and engagement in cognitively stimulating social and leisure activities, have been related to reduced risk of developing dementia, delayed onset time of AD progression, and a slower rate of cognitive decline in late adulthood (Barulli & Stern, 2013; Stern, 2009, 2012).

The cognitive reserve and brain reserve hypotheses provide a comprehensive framework at behavioral and neural levels, respectively. However, this notion seems to imply that the reserve modulates or influences aging process separately at different levels of analysis, from cognition to brain, and from morphological to functional changes. Given the cognitive neuroscience perspective that all behaviors and cognition are the outcome of brain-based information processing, the concept of a reserve is not just behavioral but also neural and, accordingly, both levels of analysis should be examined in tandem. Therefore, more recently, Cabeza et al. (2018) posited the concept of “neurocognitive reserve (NCR)” to provide a more integrative framework to understand age-related and individual differences in psychological, cognitive, and brain structure as well as neural functions (Cabeza et al., 2018).

The model of the neurocognitive reserve hypothesis has successfully provided a theoretical account for the disjunction between the degree of brain damage/pathology and the clinical manifestations of age-related and individual differences in brain structure and function. In the next section, we will discuss typical indicators or proxies to measure neurocognitive reserve at an individual level and how these indicators interact with age-related and individual differences in cognition, brain structure, and neural function.

3.2 Measures of neurocognitive reserve

A variety of measures may help us to disassociate or even predict individual variation in the neurocognitive reserve. Epidemiological evidence from neurological patients with AD and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) have shown that higher levels of verbal and non-verbal intelligence and higher levels of education/occupational attainment are good indices of neurocognitive reserve as well as good predictors of individual differences in the ability to cope with brain pathological conditions and damage before demonstrating cognitive functional deficit (usually assessed by administrating a battery of neuropsychological tests). Growing evidence supports the notion that people with higher scores on indices of reserve cope with age- and disease-related neurocognitive changes better than those with lower reserve indices (Steffener & Stern, 2012; Stern, 2012). Moreover, the levels of reserve predict the progression of cognitive decline in individuals with neurological conditions (Soldan et al., 2015; Stern, 2012; Tucker & Stern, 2011). For example, AD patients with more education and greater engagement in leisure activities (i.e., higher reserve) had less neurocognitive decline (Scarmeas & Stern, 2004). By contrast, low reserve would be a vulnerability factor for increased clinical symptoms of disease and functional limitations (Barnett, Salmond, Jones, & Sahakian, 2006; Meng & D'Arcy, 2012; Phillips, Ladouceur, & Drevets, 2008). Indeed, lower levels of occupational complexity were associated with a 2.25 times higher risk of developing dementia in late adulthood, while high participation in leisure activities was associated with a 38% lower risk of dementia (Stern, 2012).

Many epidemiological studies have shown that educational and occupational attainment, leisure activity, and life experiences are good measures/indicators of neurocognitive reserve as well as predictors of individuals who can sustain more brain damage before demonstrating cognitive functional deficit. Here, we briefly review three NCR indicators: (1) educational level; (2) occupational complexity; and (3) engagement in cognitively stimulating leisure activities.

3.2.1 Indicators of NCR: Educational level and occupational complexity

A number of cross-sectional behavioral studies have been conducted to examine the relationship between the three common indicators of NCR and neurocognitive functions in healthy older adults. For educational level, years of schooling has been used as a proxy measure of NCR and related to cognitive decline, brain atrophy, and neural function in late adulthood, with the observation that highly educated individuals show better cognitive performance as evaluated by neuropsychological assessment and larger cortical thickness as well as regional brain volume (Liu et al., 2012; Mungas, Reed, Farias, & Decarli, 2009; Opdebeeck, Martyr, & Clare, 2015; Staff, Murray, Deary, & Whalley, 2004;). However, the relationship of this proxy measure with cognitive function appears to differ across different cognitive domains, with a strong correlation between educational level and several measures of memory (e.g., short-term memory, long-term memory and relational memory) (Angel, Fay, Bouazzaoui, Baudouin, & Isingrini, 2010; Lee, Lee, & Yang, 2012) but a weak correlation between educational level and several measures of executive functions (e.g., inhibitory control, working memory, and task switching) (Lee et al., 2012; Mueller, Raymond, & Yochim, 2013).

Similarly, behavioral studies examining the association between occupational status as a proxy measure of NCR and different types of cognitive performance with age have demonstrated that higher levels of complexity of occupation are related to better cognitive performance in healthy older people (Opdebeeck et al., 2015), with a moderate correlation with executive functions (Foubert-Samier et al., 2012) and a weak correlation with memory performance (Fritsch et al., 2007). Despite the fact that these findings are congruent with the notion of neurocognitive reserve, which posits that occupational status may provide a reserve capacity of the detrimental effects of biological aging and neural diseases on neurocognitive functions (Stern, 2002, 2009), the mechanism underlying how occupational complexity impacts the reserve system and modulates neurocognitive function is unclear thus far. One explanation is the cognitive aspect of occupational attainment. Given that executive planning and organizational skills appear to be important to maintaining functional independence, individuals who have higher levels of occupational demands may engage a variety of higher-order aspects of cognition, such as working memory, to actively maintain, manipulate, and monitor information flow during work. Alternatively, the social aspect of occupational complexity posits that a higher level of occupational demands usually couples with greater levels of social engagement in the work environment, resulting in frequent social activities, demanding processes related to emotional regulation, and controlled retrieval of long-term memory, all of which have been identified to be important determinants of later-life cognitive functioning.

3.2.2 Indicators of NCR: Cognitively stimulating leisure activities

Staying engaged in cognitively stimulating leisure activities has been reported to be associated with better cognitive functioning and a reduced risk of developing Alzheimer's disease in late life (Hultsch, Hertzog, Small, & Dixon, 1999; Shimamura, Berry, Mangels, Rusting, & Jurica, 1995), suggesting that sustained practice in mental activity (e.g., reading books, attending a play, doing volunteer work, and/or engaging in social interaction) can protect against age-related cognitive declines, attenuate neural dysfunction, and postpone the onset of dementia (Chan, Haber, Drew, & Park, 2014; Carlson et al., 2008, 2009). For example, in a multi-modal, cognitively-stimulating activity program, conducted in a community-based setting, Carlson et al. (2008) randomized 149 older adults into the program and control groups. The participants who were assigned to the program team were instructed to participate in a social service program, consisting of actively working with elementary school children on reading achievement, library support, and classroom behavior for at least 15 h per week during an academic year (4–8 months). The neuropsychological assessment included measurements of memory, executive function, and psychomotor speed at baseline and follow-up phases to examine the effects of the activity program. This demonstrated that older individuals with low income, low education and at high risk for accelerated development of cognitive impairment showed significantly improved executive function and memory performance, suggesting the beneficial effects of short-term cognitively stimulating activities on cognitive function in at least some populations of older adults. Moreover, Carlson et al. (2009) used a follow-up fMRI study to investigate whether such activities could yield functional changes in the aging brain. They demonstrated that the older adults who actively participated in this short-term cognitively stimulating social service showed increased brain activity in the left prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex over the 6-month program interval (Carlson et al., 2009).

These behavioral and neuroimaging findings support the hypothesis that intellectual and social engagement that requires cognitive effort has long-term benefits for older adults, and, more importantly, provides promising evidence of neurocognitive plasticity in later adulthood. Moreover, recent behavioral studies examining the association between cognitively stimulating leisure activities as a proxy measure of NCR and cognitive performance with age have demonstrated a moderate correlation with executive function (Eskes et al., 2010; Lin, Friedman, Quinn, Chen, & Mapstone, 2012) but non-significant correlations with memory (Lin et al., 2012; Murphy, & O ’ Leary, 2010). These results imply that cognitively stimulating leisure activities as one of the Indicators of NCR may play a more domain-specific role in cognitive function.

3.2.3 Is bilingualism an indicator of NCR?

In addition to the typical protective factors discussed above, a number of researchers have suggested that lifelong language experiences (e.g., bilingualism, literacy, and verbal fluency, etc.) could also be protective factors and serve as an indicator of NCR, mitigating against age-related neurocognitive decline in normal aging (Grant, Dennis, & Li, 2014). There is evidence that bilinguals develop a higher-level control mechanism that allows them to function in one language at a time, while still maintaining the ability to switch between languages (Lee & Tzeng, 2016). Different language systems present sharp contrasts along several dimensions, including orthography, phonology, semantics, and grammar. For example, in many Indo-European languages, nouns are marked for gender, number, and definiteness, while verbs are marked for aspect, tense, and number. In Chinese, most of these grammatical markers for nouns and verbs are absent. Another example is that Chinese has a more flexible word order compared with English, which is more rigidly fixed in its SVO order. As a result, word order variation in Mandarin Chinese poses a great challenge to English learners who learn Chinese as a second language.

Bilingualism may play an important role at older ages, potentially protecting against age-associated cognitive decline. Research with older adults has shown that, in older adulthood, lifelong bilinguals' neurocognitive abilities are advantaged relative to their monolingual counterparts (Gold, 2015; Gold, Johnson, & Powell, 2013; Schweizer, Ware, Fischer, Craik, & Bialystok, 2012). The simultaneous use of two different languages has also been demonstrated to be associated with functional brain changes and different connectivity patterns. It has been shown, for example, that bilingual older adults have higher white matter integrity (Luk, Bialystok, Craik, & Grady, 2011) and gray matter intensity (Mechelli et al., 2004) than monolinguals. Furthermore, bilinguals also showed a 4- to 5-year delay in the onset of symptoms of Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer's disease (AD) compared to monolinguals (Bialystok et al., 2007; Craik, Bialystok, & Freedman, 2010). This could be due to the constant engagement of the executive control system to monitor, select, and switch between multiple languages across the lifespan. However, precisely how bilingual experiences contribute to neurocognitive reserve at the neural level is still poorly understood thus far.

Beyond bilingualism in particular, it has been suggested that literacy and/or verbal fluency may be better indicators of cognitive reserve than educational attainment (Manly, Schupf, Tang, & Stern, 2005; Manly, Touradji, Tang, & Stern, 2003). Research has uncovered important individual differences among older adults in their patterns of neural recruitment and use of language comprehension processes associated with literacy acquisition and verbal fluency. For example, higher verbal fluency (rapid cued generation) predicts increased likelihood of young-like response patterns during comprehension (Federmeier et al., 2010). Taken together, language abilities continue to reflect cerebral changes throughout a lifespan, and are important indicators of cognitive health for the elderly.

3.3 Neural mechanisms of reserve

Stern (2006, 2009, 2012) extend the cognitive aspects of the reserve hypothesis and identify two neural mechanisms that could be used to investigate the neural implementation of reserve: neural reserve and neural compensation. The mechanisms of neural reserve and neural compensation represent the structural and functional scaffolding of neurocognitive reserve, respectively.

3.3.1 Neural reserve: Structural scaffolding of cognition with age

Neural reserve refers to existing brain networks and brain structures that are more efficient, with greater capacity for coping with brain damage or increased task demands. Individuals with higher levels of NCR (e.g., higher educational level) show more efficient brain networks or greater brain capacity and tend to be less susceptible to disruption. Therefore, the concept of neural reserve provides a theoretical way to explore how NCR influences age-related and individual differences in brain structure and brain network through advanced structural neuroimaging techniques, such as anatomical MRI, diffusion MRI, Computerized Tomography (CT), etc. Individuals with higher levels of NCR should be able to better cope with age-related changes, resulting in less cognitive decline and reduced incidence of dementia with age. Here, we propose that the mechanism of neural reserve could be a structural scaffolding of neurocognitive reserve to protect against the impact of brain anatomical declines (e.g., brain volume, gray-matter density, neural myelination) and shrinkage.

We are aware that neural reserve may be shaped by an individual's innate capacity and life experience (Barulli & Stern, 2013; Stern, 2009). Genetic and environmental factors may afford some individuals with more efficient networks (such that they require less activity to achieve the same behavioral performance, due to intact brain structure), higher capacity networks (such that they have more resources available to recruit as task demands increase), or greater flexibility in brain network selection in response to cognitive challenges.

3.3.2 Neural compensation: Functional scaffolding of cognition with age

On the other hand, the mechanism of neural compensation refers to alterations in cognitive processing and compensatory brain function in response to cognitive challenges and/or brain pathology. Here, we propose that neural compensation is a functional scaffolding of neurocognitive reserve that contrasts with the structural scaffolding of neurocognitive reserve (i.e., neural reserve). We should note that both mechanisms are complimentary and testable if we employ different neuroimaging techniques. Neural compensation could be observed by measuring functional recruitment of neural resources (e.g., fMRI) in response to age-related declines in brain structure, including gray matter shrinkage, white matter reduction, or connectivity changes, which, in turn, could be quantified by measuring brain volume, gray-matter density, and neural myelination.

There is a wealth of literature showing that the functional brain ages in a dynamic way, declining in some respects but maintaining the ability to engage adaptive neural functions even in advanced age (Dennis & Cabeza, 2008; Huang, Meyer, & Federmeier, 2012; Huang, Polk, Goh, & Park, 2012; Park & Reuter-Lorenz, 2009). Thus, neural compensation is a system that is actively engaged to maintain or improve cognitive performance to accomplish cognitive tasks through the recruitment of additional, distributed, and compensatory neural networks. Previous studies have demonstrated that healthy older adults with higher levels of NCR are more able to recruit neural resources and additional, alternative neural networks in the face of brain changes due to normal aging or disease-related neurodegeneration in pathological aging (Alexander et al., 1997; Perneczky et al., 2006). These findings suggest that those elderly populations with higher levels of reserve are more capable of recruiting alternative and/or additional brain networks in response to cognitive changes after pathological aging, especially within the prefrontal regions (Korsnes & Ulstein, 2014; Scarmeas & Stern, 2004; Stern, Gazes, et al., 2018), suggesting a reserve-associated neural compensatory mechanism for age-related neurological disorders.

We are aware that a few hypotheses regarding neural compensatory processing/mechanism have been proposed in the cognitive and clinical neuroscience of aging, as well as in neuro-rehabilitation (e.g., stroke). We will next review some of the hypotheses that take this into account and that may thus relate to neurocognitive reserve.

3.3.3 Neural compensation, the CRUNCH hypothesis, and the GOLDEN view of the aging brain

The Compensation-Related Utilization of Neural Circuits Hypothesis (CRUNCH) was proposed to explain how compensatory mechanisms are modulated by different levels of task demands (Reuter-Lorenz & Cappell, 2008). The model postulates that, due to age-related decline in neural processing and neural efficiency, older adults will recruit greater levels of neural activity, while maintaining task performance, at low levels of task demands, but will show age-related decreases in neural activity at higher levels of task demand, where older adults' performance will also suffer because a resource ceiling has been reached (Reuter-Lorenz & Cappell, 2008; Reuter-Lorenz & Lustig, 2005). Moreover, CRUNCH suggests that, compared to young adults, older adults have limited functional processing resources and reach their limitation of processing earlier, resulting in a less flexible modulation of neural activity in response to increasing task demands. This notion is consistent with the concept of neurocognitive reserve, which predicts that older adults with lower level of reserve (i.e., limited functional processing resources in CRUNCH) would have a reduced capacity/efficiency of their neural reserve, resulting in less neural compensatory activation in response to increasing task demands. In addition to explaining age-related patterns, the pattern of CRUNCH could also be observed at the level of individual differences. Therefore, processing inefficiency would cause low performing individuals (whether they be young, middle-aged, or older adults) to recruit additional neural resources at low levels of task demand. As the level of task demand became higher, the high performing individuals would also need to engage additional neural resources to achieve the task goal, possibly resulting in additional, more distributed, compensatory activity even in young individuals.

Previous fMRI studies demonstrated that individual differences in working memory capacity are an important predictor of individual differences in fronto-parietal activation. For example, Schneider-Garces et al. (2010) utilized a modified version of the Sternberg memory task with five levels of working memory set size (2–6 letters in the memory set). They demonstrated that, averaged across memory set size, older adults showed poorer performance and greater neural activation in fronto-parietal regions than young adults. In addition, when performing trend analyses separately at low (set size 2–4) and high (set size 4–6) levels of memory demand, young adults were able to increase prefrontal activity as the set size increased whereas older adults were engaging more prefrontal activity at lower set sizes, which did not further increase, and even decreased, at higher set sizes. However, further analysis demonstrated that once individual differences in performance (i.e., subjective levels of task demands) were accounted for, older and young adults showed similar patterns of brain activation (Schneider-Garces et al., 2010). These results provide support for the idea that age-related functional activation in neural efficiency and capacity could stem from individual differences in working memory capacity.

It should be noted here that the findings described above (i.e., Schneider-Garces et al., 2010) also support the idea of the GOLDEN aging framework advanced by Fabiani (2012). The Growing of Life Differences Explains Normal Aging (GOLDEN) model was proposed to interpret the behavioral and neuroimaging results associated with normal aging processes. GOLDEN emphasizes that normal aging represents maturational processes resulting in progressive shifts in the distribution of mental abilities and brain activation with age (Fabiani, 2012), including reductions in working memory capacity, impairment of inhibitory control, and an increase in prefrontal activation. Moreover, GOLDEN suggests that age-related neural efficiency and capacity might stem from individual differences in working memory capacity. Given the neurocognitive reserve hypothesis' suggestion that age-related and individual differences in the efficiency and capacity of the neurocognitive network are associated with individual variations in life exposure (e.g., educational and occupational attainment, engagement in cognitively stimulating leisure activities, and bilingualism), the GOLDEN model seems to be congruent with the view of neurocognitive reserve that normal healthy aging involves the continuation of processes that are already present earlier in life and that continually shape the function and structure of the brain over time (Barulli & Stern, 2013; Martins, Joanette, & Monchi, 2015). The GOLDEN model, however, is more conservative in predicting cognitive as well as neural changes when neurodegenerative disease impacts cognitive impairments beyond normal aging processes.

3.3.4 Limitations of the reserve hypothesis

Despite the fact that the neurocognitive reserve hypothesis provides a theoretical account for the disjunction between the degree of brain damage/pathology and clinical manifestations of age-related and individual differences in brain structure and function, there are still some limitations and unanswered questions in this model. First, it is hard to quantify and weigh different NCR indicators. Several factors are attributed to the reserve, including educational level, occupational complexity, and leisure activity. However, these factors might involve different life experiences that are hard to quantify. For example, when examining the impact of education on lifetime cognitive abilities, two participants with identical educational levels may not have equivalent levels of behavioral performance, brain reserve, and neural compensation because each individual shows diverse learning curves and test performance in different disciplinary areas (mathematics, science, and literature). Moreover, it could be even more difficult to quantify the effects of education when different educational settings and countries have to be compared. Second, there is no distinct and clear definitions for these specific measures. For instance, leisure activity is overly simplified and includes almost all social and human activities. According to Stern (2009), leisure activity includes “knitting or music or other hobby, walking for pleasure or excursion, visiting friends or relatives, being visited by relatives or friends, physical conditioning, going to movies or restaurants or sporting events, reading magazines or newspapers or books, watching television or listening to the radio, doing unpaid community volunteer work, playing cards or games or bingo, going to a club or center, going to classes, and going to church or synagogue or temple.” Therefore, it becomes even harder to dissociate the influences of different life experiences and their contributions to the reserve. Finally, the process of the reserve is still unknown. How does a person's life experiences and lifestyle “store up” his or her brain or cognitive reserve? The two components of reserve (including neural reserve and neural compensation) seem like post hoc observation of the outcomes of epidemiological and brain imaging studies. These components show how the aging brain copes with brain damage, but provide no clues about the progress of the reserve. For example, how do our life experiences shape our brain network and cognitive functioning and determine the degree of the reserve? What are the moderating factors for developing neural reserve and neural compensation? What is the interaction between the brain reserve and cognitive reserve?

Moreover, there is thus far no clear evidence from longitudinal investigations of reserve and brain atrophy and neural compensation about premorbid intelligence, education, and the rate of structural changes.

3.4 The scaffolding theory of aging and cognition (STAC)

The concepts of reserve advanced by Stern (2009) refer to cognitive processes and strategies that could be beneficial for neurocognitive functions in older adults based on individual differences in lifetime experiences. However, the concept of reserve suggests that brain capacity/efficiency is fixed over time and lacks a theoretical examination of how the healthy and/or pathological aging brain can be changed when training programs and interventions are applied. The Scaffolding Theory of Aging and Cognition (STAC) has been proposed to suggest that cognitive ability is maintained at a relatively high level during normal aging, even in the face of neural challenges (such as brain damage) and brain deterioration, due to the engagement of compensatory scaffolding as a dynamic and adaptive neural system (Park & Reuter-Lorenz, 2009; Reuter-Lorenz & Park, 2014). Moreover, the STAC model incorporates the possibility that some explicit, formal, structured interventions and training programs, including learning new skills, social/intellectual engagement in late life, physical exercise, and cognitive training, could potentially have a direct influence on brain structure and function, resulting in better scaffolding activity and compensatory processing across the lifespan (Park & Reuter-Lorenz, 2009).

A revised model of STAC incorporates life-course factors that serve to enrich neural resources (e.g., intellectual engagement, education, and fitness, etc.) or deplete neural resources (e.g., Apolipoprotein E, APOEɛ4 allele), thereby influencing the developmental course of brain structure, function, and metabolism, as well as overall cognition, over time (i.e., STAC-r; Reuter-Lorenz & Park, 2014). The revised STAC model posits that age-related changes and individual differences in the level of neurocognitive function are the consequences of interactive effects from two factors: neural resource enrichment and neural resource depletion. Neural resource enrichment serves to (1) enhance/preserve brain structure and function by promoting efficient brain connectivity, increasing cortical thickness, and other indicators of brain health, and (2) increase/improve the capacity for compensatory scaffolding in the face of neural degeneration that occurs in normal and pathological aging. In contrast, neural resource depletion constitutes negative effects (e.g., depression, stress, vascular diseases) on brain connectivity, structure, and function, as well as cognition. For example, some genotypes, such as the carrier of the Apolipoprotein E (APOE) ɛ4 allele, has been shown to be highly associated with both early and late onsetting dementia (Garatachea et al., 2014; Liu et al., 2014). Both the STAC model and STAC-r are conceptual models of the cognitive neuroscience of aging that integrate evidence from genetics and structural and functional neuroimaging to explain how the combined effects of adverse and compensatory neural processes produce varying levels of cognitive function.

While the STAC-r model is similar to the concept of the reserve hypothesis advanced by Stern (Barulli & Stern, 2013; Stern, 2012), the STAC-r model provides a more positive view of the aging mind: age-related and individual differences in behavior and cognition could be enhanced and/or maintained even in advanced age by the dynamic and adaptive plasticity of the aging brain.

4 Reserve and the healthy aging brain

4.1 Is the effect of reserve specific for neurological deficits?

We should note that, despite growing interest in how reserve impacts the relationship between age-related neurological diseases (e.g., Alzheimer's disease) and observed cognitive decline, it is unclear whether the concept of reserve would show similar effects on psychological and/or psychiatric conditions, especially for elderly populations with significant health risk factors (Deschamps, 2018; Giogkaraki, Michaelides, & Constantinidou, 2013; Tucker & Stern, 2011; Watson & Joyce, 2015). Therefore, it is reasonable to posit that neurocognitive reserve should be a protective mechanism to facilitate cognitive performance and decrease age-related and disease-associated clinical symptoms following many types of brain pathologies and/or brain damage (Barnett et al., 2006; Stern, 2007, 2009, 2012; Turner & Lloyd, 1999; Venezia et al., 2018). In other words, higher reserve would appear to be a protective factor against the symptoms of all brain-based clinical conditions, including not only age-related neurological diseases (e.g., Alzheimer's disease, traumatic brain injury, etc.) but also psychiatric diseases (e.g., late-life depression, schizophrenia, etc.) (Barnett et al., 2006; Turner & Lloyd, 1999; Venezia et al., 2018).

Some previous epidemiological and behavioral studies have provided evidence for this notion using multiple measures of neurocognitive reserve. There are behavioral results demonstrating that levels of education in later life influence the risk of an individual's depression severity (Ladin, 2008; Spitznagel, Tremont, Brown, & Gunstad, 2006; Turner & Lloyd, 1999). Barnett et al. (2006) further suggested that educational levels and occupational attainment influence cognitive performance and the severity of psychiatric symptoms in other neuropathological disorders, such as schizophrenia (Barnett et al., 2006; de la Serna et al., 2013; Munro, Russell, Murray, Kerwin, & Jones, 2002), bipolar disorder, and depression (Barnett et al., 2006). Some other studies have similar findings in patients with HIV (Farinpour et al., 2003) and traumatic brain injury (Stern, 2009).

These findings from psychiatric research suggest that neurocognitive reserve may not be encoded in neurological-specific ways for elderly populations. Notably, neurocognitive reserve is relevant not just to the pathological aging process but also to the normal aging process, with the idea that NCR mechanisms may allow people to cope more effectively with a variety of age- and disease-related cognitive decline and functional changes (i.e., CRUNCH and STAC-r).

Next, we discuss emerging functional and structural neuroimaging evidence from geriatric depression (i.e., late life depression) to support the notion that neurocognitive reserve may play a protective role for improving the resilience and adaptability of the aging brain to cope with psychiatric conditions.

4.2 Aging, reserve and brain health: An example of geriatric depression

Major depressive disorder (MDD), a leading cause of disability worldwide, is a devastating mental illness affecting one's mental well-being and brain health (Moussavi et al., 2007). With the trend of an aging population, an increasing prevalence of late-life depression (LLD) has been identified (Beekman, Copeland, & Prince, 1999). Moreover, more than one-third of LLD patients cannot attain full remission after antidepressant treatments (for review, see Nelson, Delucchi, & Schneider, 2008). The devastating effects of LLD on the psychological and brain health of older people have been reported, and include loss of energy, increase in suicide, declined neurocognitive performance, and dysfunction of brain networks and functioning (Lam et al., 2018; Lin et al., 2019; Sin et al., 2018; for review, see Tadayonnejad & Ajilore, 2013).

There is converging evidence suggesting that psychiatric patients with late-life depression have reduced gray matter and white matter volume in the medial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), corpus callosum fibers, and limbic system (mainly in hippocampus and amygdala) (Treadway et al., 2015). Moreover, such structural abnormalities were associated with functional changes in the brain network related to cognitive control, emotional regulation, and working memory (McEwen, 2007). Therefore, if NCR is a general principle describing individual differences in brain health and adaptability, then it should also serve as a protective factor for consequences of geriatric depression.

Although some studies failed to demonstrate the beneficial effects of neurocognitive reserve on behavioral performance in individuals with LLD (Bhalla et al., 2005), a study conducted by O'Shea et al. (2015) reported that reading ability as a reserve indicator significantly influences the association between depressive symptoms and executive functioning in individuals with LLD (O'Shea et al., 2015). In one of our task-related functional MRI studies, we investigated whether and how various levels of education and verbal capacity (two widely used proxies for reserve) impact the adverse influence of age-related psychiatric conditions on severity of depression, behavioral performance, and neural processing of emotional control in patients with LLD (Huang et al., in press). Fifty older adults suffering from late-life depression (LLD group) and 37 age-matched healthy elderly controls (HEC group) performed the modified version of an emotional Stroop task (eStroop task) during fMRI scanning to tap their behavioral performance and neural processing of selective attention, inhibition of emotional responses and conflict resolutions (Dalgleish & Watts, 1990; Williams, Mathews, & MacLeod, 1996). In the emotional Stroop task, participants were instructed to indicate the ink color of target words that are either emotionally-salient or neutral. The target word is evocative of a neutral emotion (e.g., “motivation”), a positive emotion (e.g., “joy”), or a negative emotion word (e.g., “sad”). Participants were required to indicate via button press whether the ink color of each target word matches the color meaning of a word below it as accurately and quickly as possible.

Behvaiorally, we found that the LLD group showed more anxiety and severity of depression than the HEC group, and performed less accurately on the eStroop task. Moreover, we found that verbal fluency was positively correlated with eStroop behavioral performance in LLD, and severity of depression was negatively related to activity within the eStroop task-evoked prefrontal regions (ACC, middle frontal cortex, and anterior insula). The results of brain-behavioral correlation analyses showed that older adults who had higher levels of neurocognitive reserve (i.e., more years of education and greater verbal fluency scores) showed less severity of depression and greater brain activity in the middle frontal cortex than those with a lower level of neurocognitive reserve. In addition, given that both GDS ratings and years of education were correlated with activity in the middle frontal cortex, and more years of education was also related to reduced severity of depression, we then performed a post hoc mediation analysis to examine whether neurocognitive reserve factors mediate the association between severity of depression and middle frontal activity. We found that more years of education and greater verbal fluency were associated with less severity of depression and increased MFG activation in response to the interference of emotional words, suggesting education and language ability were effective mediators in the association between severity of depression and the neural activity within frontal cortex for processing emotional interference.

Our behavioral and functional neuroimaging findings provide evidence for the protective effects of reserve upon cognition in geriatric depression. This finding also supports the notion that when brain pathology and/or damage alters the task-related processing networks, individuals with higher levels of neurocognitive reserve are able to utilize alternative or additional neural mechanisms to cope with brain pathology (i.e., neural compensation) (Stern, 2009, 2012).

In another neuroimaging study conducted by our lab and our collaborators, we utilized structural MRI to identify whether and how various levels of education (proxy for reserve) interact with neurocognitive function, severity of depression, and white matter hyperintensities in later life depression. White matter hyperintensities (WMH) are neuroimaging-defined vascular changes and have been reported to be essential neural markers of vascular changes in late-life depression (Krishnan, Hays, & Blazer, 1997; Taylor, Aizenstein, & Alexopoulos, 2013). Previous studies suggested that WMH may stem from neuronal demyelination, gliosis, and axonal loss in periventricular or deep white-matter regions (Wardlaw, Hernández, & Muñoz-Maniega, 2015). Moreover, WMH is not only observed in the normal aging population but is also associated with the onset of late life depression (Herrmann, Le Masurier, & Ebmeier, 2008) and with the risk of subsequent stroke, dementia, and death (Debette & Markus, 2010). Given the idea that neurocognitive reserve may be encoded in more general rather than disorder-specific ways for the elderly population, we hypothesized that neurocognitive reserve would modify the impact of WMH on depressive symptoms and a variety of cognitive functions. A total of 54 elderly people with LLD and 38 matched healthy controls participated in this MRI study. Our findings demonstrated that the relationship of WMH on depressive symptoms and cognitive functions differs by education level in patients with LLD. The level of education may mitigate the negative association between WMH, depressive symptoms, and language function. This implies that higher levels of neurocognitive reserve, by proxy of education level, could defy the deleterious effect of WMH in LLD (Lin et al., under review).

In conclusion, these behavioral and neuroimaging findings provide evidence to support the notion that the levels of neurocognitive reserve may serve as a protective mechanism that increases the brain's capacity to cope with age-related psychiatric disorder. The results demonstrated the beneficial effects of neurocognitive reserve on improving behavioral performance, reducing clinical symptoms, enhancing neural processing, and scaffolding neural resilience with late-life depression. Finally, these studies complement the existing literature on the role of neurocognitive reserve in both healthy and pathological aging.

5 Conclusion and new directions

There is converging evidence that neurocognitive reserve shapes age-related and individual differences in neurocognitive processes, influences patterns of neural activation, and may even sculpt brain structure and neural network organization. The concept of neurocognitive reserve provides a theoretical framework that individual differences in lifelong experiences could impact the nature of resilience and adaptability of the aging brain to cope with brain pathology and damage. An important direction for investigations of neurocognitive reserve will be to develop broader frameworks that go beyond normal aging and pathological aging and to consistently consider the effectiveness of neuro-rehabilitation and/or geriatric rehabilitation programs for treatment responses and prognosis after pathological aging. This research is a critical domain for understanding the characteristics of aging-related changes in our neurocognitive functions, and more importantly, understanding the adaptive nature of the aging brain. Both are essential for paving the way for successful aging at the individual level and for designing policies and environments that are friendly to the aged society.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Academia Sinica Thematic Research Program (AS-103-TP-C04) and Ministry of Science and Technology Grant (105-2420-H-009-001-MY2; 107-2410-H-009-028 -MY3) 5-2420-H-009-001-MY2) in Taiwan.

References

Alexander G.E., Furey M.L., Grady C.L., Pietrini P., Brady D.R., Mentis M.J., et al. Association of premorbid intellectual function with cerebral metabolism in Alzheimer's disease: Implications for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 1997;154(2):165–172.

Alzheimer's Association. Alzheimer's disease facts and figures. Alzheimer's & Dementia. 2015. ;11(3):332–384. 2015 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2015.02.003.

Andrews-Hanna J.R., et al. Disruption of large-scale brain systems in advanced aging. Neuron. 2007;56:924–935.

Angel L., Fay S., Bouazzaoui B., Baudouin A., Isingrini M. Protective role of educational level on episodic memory aging: An event-related potential study. Brain and Cognition. 2010;74:312–323.

Baltes P.B., Lindenberger U. Emergence of a powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: A new window to the study of cognitive aging?. Psychology and Aging. 1997;12:12–21.

Barnett J.H., Salmond C.H., Jones P.B., Sahakian B.J. Cognitive reserve in neuropsychiatry. Psychological Medicine. 2006;36(8):1053–1064. doi:10.1017/S0033291706007501.

Barulli D., Stern Y. Efficiency, capacity, compensation, maintenance, plasticity: Emerging concepts in cognitive reserve. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2013;17(10):502–509.

Beekman A.T.F., Copeland J., Prince M.J. Review of community prevalence of depression in later life. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;174(4):307–311. doi:10.1192/bjp.174.4.307.

Bennett I.J., Madden D.J. Disconnected aging: Cerebral white matter integrity and age-related differences in cognition. Neuroscience. 2014;276:187–205.

Bhalla R.K., Butters M.A., Zmuda M.D., Seligman K., Mulsant B.H., Pollock B.G., et al. Does education moderate neuropsychological impairment in late-life depression?. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2005;20(5):413–417. doi:10.1002/gps.1296.

Bialystok E., Craik F.I.M., Freedman M. Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(2):459–464.

Bialystok E., Klein R., Craik F.I.M., Viswanathan M. Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging. 2004;19(2):290–303.

Bopp K.L., Verhaeghen P. Aging and verbal memory span: A meta-analysis. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2005;60:223–233.

Bugg J.M., Head D. Exercise moderates age-related atrophy of the medial temporal lobe. Neurobiology of Aging. 2011;32(3):506–514. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.03.008.

Burns A., Iliffe S. Alzheimer's disease. BMJ. 2009;338:b158. doi:10.1136/bmj.b158.

Cabeza R., Albert M., Belleville S., Craik F.I.M., Duarte A., Grady C.L., et al. Maintenance, reserve and compensation: The cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2018;19(11):701–710. doi:10.1038/s41583-018-0068-2.

Cabeza R., Anderson N.D., Locantore J.K., McIntosh A.R. Aging gracefully: Compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. NeuroImage. 2002;17:1394–1402.

Cabeza R., Daselaar S.M., Dolcos F., Prince S.E., Budde M., Nyberg L. Task-independent and task-specific age effects on brain activity during working memory, visual attention and episodic retrieval. Cerebral Cortex. 2004;14:364–375.

Carlson M.C., Erickson K.I., Kramer A.F., Voss M.W., Bolea N., Mielke M., et al. Evidence for neurocognitive plasticity in at-risk older adults: The experience corps program. The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2009;64:1275–1282.

Carlson M.C., Saczynski J.S., Rebok G.W., Seeman T., Glass T.A., McGill S., et al. Exploring the effects of an “Everyday” activity program on executive function and memory in older adults: Experience Corps®. The Gerontologist. 2008;48(6):793–801. doi:10.1093/geront/48.6.793.

Chan M.Y., Haber S., Drew L.M., Park D.C. Training older adults to use tablet computers: Does it enhance cognitive function?. The Gerontologist. 2014;56(3):475–484. doi:10.1093/geront/gnu057.

Charlton R.A., Barrick T.R., McIntyre D.J., Shen Y., O’ Sullivan M., Howe F.A., et al. White matter damage on diffusion tensor imaging correlates with age-related cognitive decline. Neurology. 2006;66:217–222.

Christensen H., Anstey K.J., Leach L.S., Mackinnon A.J. Intelligence, education, and the brain reserve hypothesis. In: The handbook of aging and cognition. 3rd ed. New York, NY, US: Psychology Press; 2008:133–188.

Cox S.R., et al. Ageing and brain white matter structure in 3,513 UK Biobank participants. Nature Communications. 2016;7:13629. doi:10.1038/ncomms13629.

Craik F.I.M., Bialystok E., Freedman M. Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease: Bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology. 2010;75(19):1726–1729.

Craik F.I.M., Byrd M. Aging and cognitive deficits: The role of attentional resources. In: Craik F.I.M., Trehub S., eds. Aging and cognitive processes. New York: Plenum Press; 1982:191–211.

Dalgleish T., Watts F.N. Biases of attention and memory in disorders of anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychology Review. 1990;10(5):589–604. doi:10.1016/0272-7358(90)90098-U.

Damoiseaux J.S., et al. Reduced resting-state brain activity in the “default network” in normal aging. Cerebral Cortex. 2008;18:1856–1864.

Davis S.W., Dennis N.A., Buchler N.G., White L.E., Madden D.J., Cabeza R. Assessing the effects of age on long white matter tracts using diffusion tensor tractography. NeuroImage. 2009;46:530–541.

Davis S.W., Dennis N.A., Daselaar S.M., Fleck M.S., Cabeza R. Que PASA? The posterior-anterior shift in aging. Cerebral Cortex. 2008;18:1201–1209.

Davis S.W., Kragel J.E., Madden D.J., Cabeza R. The Architecture of cross-hemispheric communication in the aging brain: Linking behavior to functional and structural connectivity. Cerebral Cortex. 2011;22:232–242. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr123.

de la Serna E., Andrés-Perpiñá S., Puig O., Baeza I., Bombin I., Bartrés-Faz D., et al. Cognitive reserve as a predictor of two year neuropsychological performance in early onset first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2013;143(1):125–131.

Debette S., Markus H.S. The clinical importance of white matter hyperintensities on brain magnetic resonance imaging: Systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ. 2010;341:c3666.

Dennis N.A., Cabeza R. Neuroimaging of healthy cognitive aging. In: Handbook of aging and cognition. New York, USA: Psychology Press; 1–54. 2008;Vol. 3.

Deschamps T. The cognitive reserve should be controlled when using neuroimaging to assess relapse in major depressive disorder. JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75(9):973. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.1477.

Dickerson B.C., Feczko E., Augustinack J.C., Pacheco J., Morris J.C., Fischl B., et al. Differential effects of aging and Alzheimer's disease on medial temporal lobe cortical thickness and surface area. Neurobiology of Aging. 2009;30:432–440.

Duzel E., Schutze H., Yonelinas A.P., Heinze H.J. Functional phenotyping of successful aging in long-term memory: Preserved performance in the absence of neural compensation. Hippocampus. 2011;21:803–814.

Elderkin-Thompson V., Ballmaier M., Hellemann G., Pham D., Kumar A. Executive function and MRI prefrontal volumes among healthy older adults. Neuropsychology. 2008;22:626–637.

Eskes G.A., Longman S., Brown A.D., McMorris C.A., Langdon K.D., Hogan D.B., et al. Contribution of physical fitness, cerebrovascular reserve and cognitive stimulation to cognitive function in post-menopausal women. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2010;2:137.

Fabiani M. It was the best of times, it was the worst of times: A psychophysiologist's view of cognitive aging. Psychophysiology. 2012;49:283–304.

Fan Y.T., Fang Y.W., Chen Y.P., Leshikar E.D., Lin C.P., Tzeng O.J.L., et al. Aging, cognition, and the brain: Effects of age-related variation in white matter integrity on neuropsychological function. Aging & Mental Health. 2019;23(7):831–839. doi:10.1080/13607863.2018.1455804.

Farinpour R., Miller E.N., Satz P., Selnes O.A., Cohen B.A., Becker J.T., et al. Psychosocial risk factors of HIV morbidity and mortality: Findings from the multicenter AIDS Cohort Study (MACS). Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2003;25(5):654–670. doi:10.1076/jcen.25.5.654.14577.

Federmeier K.D. Thinking ahead: The role and roots of prediction in language comprehension. Psychophysiology. 2007;44(4):491–505.

Federmeier K.D., Kutas M. Aging in context: Age-related changes in context use during language comprehension. Psychophysiology. 2005;42:133–141.

Federmeier K.D., Kutas M., Schul R. Age-related and individual differences in the use of prediction during language comprehension. Brain and Language. 2010;115(3):149–216.

Federmeier K.D., McLennan D.B., De Ochoa E., Kutas M. The impact of semantic memory organization and sentence context information on spoken language processing by younger and older adults: An ERP study. Psychophysiology. 2002;39(2):133–146.

Federmeier K.D., Van Petten C., Schwartz T.J., Kutas M. Sounds, words, sentences: Age-related changes across levels of language processing. Psychology and Aging. 2003;18:858–872.

Fjell A.M., Walhovd K.B., Reinvang I., Lundervold A., Salat D., Quinn B.T., et al. Selective increase of cortical thickness in high-performing elderly-structural indices of optimal cognitive aging. NeuroImage. 2006;29:984–994.

Fjell A.M., Westlye L.T., Amlien I., Espeseth T., Reinvang I., Raz N., et al. High consistency of regional cortical thinning in aging across multiple samples. Cerebral Cortex. 2009;19:2001–2012.

Foubert-Samier A., Catheline G., Amieva H., Dilharreguy B., Helmer C., Allard M., et al. Education, occupation, leisure activities, and brain reserve: A population-based study. Neurobiology of Aging. 2012;33:423.e15–423.e25.

Fritsch T., McClendon M.J., Smyth K.A., Lerner A.J., Friedland R.P., Larsen J.D. Cognitive functioning in healthy aging: The role of reserve and lifestyle factors early in life. The Gerontologist. 2007;47(3):307–322. doi:10.1093/geront/47.3.307.

Garatachea N., Emanuele E., Calero M., Fuku N., Arai Y., Abe Y., et al. ApoE gene and exceptional longevity: Insights from three independent cohorts. Experimental Gerontology. 2014;53:16–23. doi:10.1016/j.exger.2014.02.004.

Giogkaraki E., Michaelides M.P., Constantinidou F. The role of cognitive reserve in cognitive aging: Results from the neurocognitive study on aging. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2013;35(10):1024–1035. doi:10.1080/13803395.2013.847906.

Goh J.O., An Y., Resnick S.M. Differential trajectories of age-related changes in components of executive and memory processes. Psychology and Aging. 2012;27(3):707–719. doi:10.1037/a0026715.

Goh J., Huang C.M. Images of the cognitive brain across age and culture. In: Neuroimaging—Cognitive and clinical neuroscience. Peter Bright, IntechOpen; 2012:doi:10.5772/23944.

Goh J.O., Suzuki A., Park D.C. Reduced neural selectivity increases fMRI adaptation with age during face discrimination. NeuroImage. 2010;51:336–344.

Gold B.T. Lifelong bilingualism and neural reserve against Alzheimer's disease: A review of findings and potential mechanisms. Behavioural Brain Research. 2015;15(281):9–15.

Gold B.T., Johnson N.F., Powell D.K. Lifelong bilingualism contributes to cognitive reserve against white matter integrity declines in aging. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51(13):2841–2846.

Gold B.T., Powell D.K., Xuan L., Jicha G.A., Smith C.D. Age-related slowing of task switching is associated with decreased integrity of frontoparietal white matter. Neurobiology of Aging. 2010;31:512–522.

Good C.D., Johnsrude I.S., Ashburner J., Henson R.N., Friston K.J., Frackowiak R.S. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. NeuroImage. 2001;14:21–36.

Gordon B., Rykhlevskaia E., Brumback C.R., Lee Y., Elavsky S., Konopack J.F., et al. Anatomical correlates of aging, cardiopulmonary fitness level, and education. Psychophysiology. 2008;45:825–838.

Grady C.L. The cognitive neuroscience of ageing. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2012;13:491–505.

Grant A., Dennis N.A., Li P. Cognitive control, cognitive reserve, and memory in the aging bilingual brain. Frontiers in Psychology. 2014;5:1401. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01401.

Gratton G., Wee E., Rykhlevskaia E.I., Leaver E.E., Fabiani M. Does white matter matter? Spatio-temporal dynamics of task switching in aging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21:13801395.

Gunning-Dixon F.M., Raz N. Neuroanatomical correlates of selected executive function in middle-aged and older adults: A prospective MRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:1929–1941.

Gutchess A.H., Welsh R.C., Hedden T., Bangert A., Minear M., Liu L.L., et al. Aging and the neural correlates of successful picture encoding: Frontal activations compensate for decreased medial-temporal activity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2005;17:84–96.

Guthrie J.T., Wigfield A., Barbosa P., Perencevich K.C., Taboada A., Davis M.H., et al. Increasing reading comprehension and engagement through concept-oriented reading instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2004;96:403–423.

Hasher L., Zacks R.T. Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In: Bower G.H., ed. The psychology of learning and motivation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 93–225. 1988;22.

Head D., Kennedy K.M., Rodrigue K.M., Raz N. Age differences in perseveration: Cognitive and neuroanatomical mediators of performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:1200–1203.

Head D., Rodrigue K.M., Kennedy K.M., Raz N. Neuroanatomical and cognitive mediators of age-related differences in episodic memory. Neuropsychology. 2008;22:491–507.

Hedden T., Park D.C., Nisbett R., Ji L.-J., Jing Q., Jiao S. Cultural variation in verbal versus spatial neuropsychological function across the life span. Neuropsychology. 2002;16(1):65–73. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.16.1.65.

Herrmann L.L., Le Masurier M., Ebmeier K.P. White matter hyperintensities in late life depression: A systematic review. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry. 2008;79:619–624.

Heuninckx S., Wenderoth N., Swinnen S.P. Systems neuroplasticity in the aging brain: Recruiting additional neural resources for successful motor performance in elderly persons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:91–99.

Howard D.V., Howard J.H., Dennis N.A., LaVine S., Valentino K. Aging and implicit learning of an invariant association. Journal of Gerontology. 2008;63:100–105.

Huang C.M., Fan Y.T., Lee S.H., Liu H.L., Lin C.M., Lee T.M.C., Cognitive reserve-mediated neural modulation of emotional interference and depressive symptoms in late-life depression, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, in press.

Huang H.W., Meyer A.M., Federmeier K.D. A “concrete view” of aging: Event related potentials reveal age-related changes in basic integrative processes in language. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50(1):26–35.

Huang C.M., Polk T.A., Goh J.O., Park D.C. Both left and right posterior parietal activations contribute to compensatory processes in normal aging. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50:55–66.

Hultsch D.F., Hertzog C., Small B.J., Dixon R.A. Use it or lose it: Engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging?. Psychology and Aging. 1999;14:245–263.

Jimura K., Braver T.S. Age-related shifts in brain activity dynamics during task switching. Cerebral Cortex. 2010;20:1420–1431.

Katzman R., Terry R., DeTeresa R., Brown T., Davies P., Fuld P., et al. Clinical, pathological, and neurochemical changes in dementia: A subgroup with preserved mental status and numerous neocortical plaques. Annals of Neurology. 1988;23(2):138–144. doi:10.1002/ana.410230206.

Kennedy K.M., Raz N. Aging white matter and cognition: Differential effects of regional variations in diffusion properties on memory, executive functions, and speed. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:916–927.

Korsnes M.S., Ulstein I.D. Cognitive effects of late life depression: Review of neuropsychological findings. Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science. 2014;4:141–157.

Krishnan K., Hays J.C., Blazer D.G. MRI-defined vascular depression. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 1997;154:497–501.

Kutas M., Federmeier K.D. Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology. 2011;62:621–647.

Ladin K. Risk of late-life depression across 10 European Union countries: Deconstructing the education effect. Journal of Aging and Health. 2008;6:653–670.

Lam C.L.M., Liu H.-L., Huang C.-M., Wai Y.-Y., Lee S.-H., Yiend J., et al. The neural correlates of perceived energy levels in older adults with late-life depression. Brain Imaging and Behavior. 2018;1–9. doi:10.1007/s11682-018-9940-y.

Lee Y.-S., Lee C.-L., Yang H.-T. Effects of aging and education on false memory. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development. 2012;74:287–298.

Lee R.R.-W., Tzeng O.J.-L. Neural Bilingualism: A new look at an old problem. Language and Linguistics. 2016;17(2):147–193.

Li S.-C. Biocultural orchestration of developmental plasticity across levels: The interplay of biology and culture in shaping the mind and behavior across the life span. Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129(2):171–194. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.171.

Lin F., Friedman E., Quinn J., Chen D., Mapstone M. Effect of leisure activities on inflammation and cognitive function in an aging sample. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics. 2012;54:e398–e404.

Lin C., Lee S.-H., Huang C.-M., Chen G.-Y., Ho P.-S., Liu H.-L., et al. Increased brain entropy of resting-state fMRI mediates the relationship between depression severity and mental health-related quality of life in late-life depressed elderly. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2019;250:270–277. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2019.03.012.

Liu Y., Julkunen V., Paajanen T., Westman E., Wahlund L.O., Aitken A., et al. Education increases reserve against Alzheimer's disease-evidence from structural MRI analysis. Neuroradiology. 2012;54:929–938.

Liu Y., et al. APOE genotype and neuroimaging markers of Alzheimer's disease: Systematic review & meta-analysis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 2014;86:127–134.

Luk G., Bialystok E., Craik F.I.M., Grady C.L. Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31(46):16808–16813.

Lutz W., Sanderson W., Scherbov S. The coming acceleration of global population ageing. Nature. 2008;451:716–719.

Madden D.J., Bennett I.J., Song A.W. Cerebral white matter integrity and cognitive aging: Contributions from diffusion tensor imaging. Neuropsychology Review. 2009;19:415–435.

Manly J.J., Schupf N., Tang M.-X., Stern Y. Cognitive decline and literacy among ethnically diverse elders. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology. 2005;18:213–217.

Manly J.J., Touradji P., Tang M.-X., Stern Y. Literacy and memory decline among ethnically diverse elders. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2003;25:680–690.

Martins R., Joanette Y., Monchi O. The implications of age-related neurofunctional compensatory mechanisms in executive function and language processing including the new Temporal Hypothesis for Compensation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2015. ;9(221). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00221.

McEwen B.S. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: Central role of the brain. Physiological Reviews. 2007;87(3):873–904. doi:10.1152/physrev.00041.2006.

Mechelli A., Crinion J.T., Noppeney U., O'Doherty J., Ashburner J., Frackowiak R.S., et al. Neurolinguistics: Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature. 2004;431:757.

Meng X., D'Arcy C. Education and dementia in the context of the cognitive reserve hypothesis: A systematic review with meta-analyses and qualitative analyses. PLoS One. 2012;7(6):e38268doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0038268.

Moussavi S., Chatterji S., Verdes E., Tandon A., Patel V., Ustun B. Depression, chronic diseases, and decrements in health: Results from the World Health Surveys. The Lancet. 2007;370(9590):851–858. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61415-9.

Mueller A.E., Raymond N., Yochim B.P. Cognitive activity engagement predicts future memory and executive functioning in older adults. Activities, Adaptation, & Aging. 2013;37:251–264.

Mungas D., Reed B.R., Farias S.T., Decarli C. Age and education effects on relationships of cognitive test scores with brain structure in demographically diverse older persons. Psychology and Aging. 2009;24:116–128.

Munro J.C., Russell A.J., Murray R.M., Kerwin R.W., Jones P.B. IQ in childhood psychiatric attendees predicts outcome of later schizophrenia at 21 year follow-up. Acta Psychiatric Scandianvica. 2002;106(2):139–142.

Murphy M., O’Leary E. Depression, cognitive reserve and memory performance in older adults. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2010;25:665–671.

Na J., Huang C.-M., Park D.C. When age and culture interact in an easy and yet cognitively demanding task: Older adults, but not younger adults, showed the expected cultural differences. Frontiers in Psychology. 2017;8:457. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00457.

Nelson J.C., Delucchi K., Schneider L.S. Efficacy of second generation antidepressants in late-life depression: A meta-analysis of the evidence. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2008;16(7):558–567. doi:10.1097/01.JGP.0000308883.64832.ed.

Nyberg L., Lövdén M., Riklund K., Lindenberger U., Bäckman L. Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012;16(5):292–305. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.005.

Opdebeeck C., Martyr A., Clare L. Cognitive reserve and cognitive function in healthy older people: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition. 2015;1:1–21.

O'Shea D.M., Fieo R.A., Hamilton J.L., Zahodne L.B., Manly J.J., Stern Y. Examining the association between late-life depressive symptoms, cognitive function, and brain volumes in the context of cognitive reserve. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2015;30(6):614–622.

Park D.C., Lautenschlager G., Hedden T., Davidson N.S., Smith A.D., Smith P.K. Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging. 2002;17:299–320.

Park D.C., Reuter-Lorenz P. The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology. 2009;60:173–196.

Park D.C., Smith A.D., Lautenschlager G., Earles J.L., Frieske D., Zwahr M., et al. Mediators of long-term memory performance across the life span. Psychology and Aging. 1996;11:621–637.

Park D.C., Welsh R.C., Marshuetz C., Gutchess A.H., Mikels J., Polk T.A., et al. Working memory for complex scenes: Age differences in frontal and hippocampal activations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2003;15:1122–1134.

Payne B.R., Gao X., Noh S.R., Anderson C.J., Stine-Morrow E.A.L. The effects of print exposure on sentence processing and memory in older adults: Evidence for efficiency and reserve. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition. 2012;19(1-2):122–149. doi:10.1080/13825585.2011.628376.

Perneczky R., Drzezga A., Diehl-Schmid J., Schmid G., Wohlschläger A., Kars S., et al. Schooling mediates brain reserve in Alzheimer's disease: Findings of fluoro-deoxy-glucose- positron emission tomography. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 2006;77(9):1060–1063. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2006.094714.

Perneczky R., Wagenpfeil S., Lunetta K.L., Cupples L.A., Green R.C., DeCarli C., et al. Head circumference, atrophy, and cognition. Implications for brain reserve in Alzheimer disease. 2010;75(2):137–142. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181e7ca97.

Persson J., Lustig C., Nelson J.K., Reuter-Lorenz P.A. Age differences in deactivation: A link to cognitive control?. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2007;19:1021–1032.

Phillips M.L., Ladouceur C.D., Drevets W.C. A neural model of voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: implications for understanding the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder. Molecular Psychiatry. 2008. ;13(9):833–857. 829 https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2008.65.

PhRMA, Medicines in development for Alzheimer's disease: 2013 Report. Available at: http://phrma-docs.phrma.org/sites/default/files/Alzheimer%27s%202013.pdf. (Accessed 16 February 2018).

Querbes O., Aubry F., Pariente J., Lotterie J.-A., Démonet J.-F., Duret V., et al. Early diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease using cortical thickness: Impact of cognitive reserve. Brain. 2009;132(8):2036–2047. doi:10.1093/brain/awp105.

Raz N., Lindenberger U., Rodrigue K.M., Kennedy K.M., Head D., Williamson A., et al. Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: General trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cerebral Cortex. 2005;15:1676–1689.

Resnick S.M., Pham D.L., Kraut M.A., Zonderman A.B., Davatzikos C. Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging studies of older adults: A shrinking brain. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:3295–3301.

Reuter-Lorenz P.A., Cappell K.A. Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2008;17:177–182.

Reuter-Lorenz P.A., Lustig C. Brain aging: Reorganizing discoveries about the aging mind. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2005;15:245–251.

Reuter-Lorenz P.A., Park D.C. How does it STAC Up? Revisiting the scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Neuropsychology Review. 2014;24:355–370.

Ritchie S.J., Bastin M.E., Tucker-Drob E.M., Maniega S.M., Engelhardt L.E., Cox S.R., et al. Coupled changes in brain white matter microstructure and fluid intelligence in later life. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35(22):8672–8682.

Rodrigue K.M., Raz N. Shrinkage of the entorhinal cortex over five years predicts memory performance in healthy adults. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:956–963.

Rowe J.W., Kahn R.L. Successful aging. Gerontologist. 1997;37:433–440 [1997].

Salat D.H., Buckner R.L., Snyder A.Z., Greve D.N., Desikan R.S.R., Busa E., et al. Thinning of the cerebral cortex in aging. Cerebral Cortex. 2004;14:721–730.

Salat D.H., Greve D.N., Pacheco J.L., Quinn B.T., Helmer K.G., Buckner R.L., et al. Regional white matter volume differences in nondemented aging and Alzheimer's disease. NeuroImage. 2009;44:1247–1258.

Salthouse T. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review. 1996;103(3):403–428.

Sambataro F., et al. Age-related alterations in default mode network: Impact on working memory performance. Neurobiology of Aging. 2010;31:839–852.

Scarmeas N., Stern Y. Cognitive reserve: Implications for diagnosis and prevention of Alzheimer's disease. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports. 2004;4(5):374–380.

Schneider-Garces N.J., Gordon B.A., Brumback-Peltz C.R., Shin E., Lee Y., Sutton B.P., et al. Span, CRUNCH, and beyond: Working memory capacity and the aging brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2010;22:655–669.

Schofield P.W., Logroscino G., Andrews H.F., Albert S., Stern Y. An association between head circumference and Alzheimer's disease in a population-based study of aging and dementia. Neurology. 1997;49(1):30–37.

Schweizer T.A., Ware J., Fischer C.E., Craik F.I., Bialystok E. Bilingualism as a contributor to cognitive reserve: Evidence from brain atrophy in Alzheimer's disease. Cortex. 2012;48(8):991–996.

Shimamura A.P., Berry J.M., Mangels J.A., Rusting C.L., Jurica P.J. Memory and cognitive abilities in university professors: Evidence for successful aging. Psychological Science. 1995;6:271–277.

Sin E.L.L., Liu H.-L., Lee S.-H., Huang C.-M., Wai Y.-Y., Chen Y.-L., et al. The relationships between brain structural changes and perceived loneliness in older adults suffering from late-life depression. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2018;33(4):606–612. doi:10.1002/gps.4831.

Smith C.D., Chebrolu H., Wekstein D.R., Schmitt F.A., Markesbery W.R. Age and gender effects on human brain anatomy: A voxel-based morphometric study in healthy elderly. Neurobiology of Aging. 2007;28:1075–1087.

Soldan A., Pettigrew C., Lu Y., Wang M.C., Selnes O., Albert M., et al. Relationship of medial temporal lobe atrophy, APOE genotype, and cognitive reserve in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Human Brain Mapping. 2015;36(7):2826–2841. doi:10.1002/hbm.22810.

Sommers M.S., Danielson S.M. Inhibitory processes and spoken word recognition in young and older adults: The interaction of lexical competition and semantic context. Psychology and Aging. 1999;14(3):458–472.

Spitznagel M.B., Tremont G., Brown L.B., Gunstad J. Cognitive reserve and the relationship between depressive symptoms and awareness of deficits in dementia. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2006;18(2):186–190.

Staff R.T., Murray A.D., Deary I.J., Whalley L.J. What provides cerebral reserve?. Brain. 2004;127:1191–1199.

Steffener J., Stern Y. Exploring the neural basis of cognitive reserve in aging. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta. 2012;1822(3):467–473.

Stern Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2002;8(3):448–460.

Stern Y. Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders. 2006;20:112–117.

Stern Y. Cognitive reserve: Theory and applications. New York: Taylor & Francis; 2007.

Stern Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(10):2015–2028. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004.

Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurology. 2012;11(11):1006–1012. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6.

Stern Y., Arenaza-Urquijo E.M., Bartres-Faz D., Belleville S., Cantilon M., Chetelat G., et al. Whitepaper: Defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimer's & Dementia: The Journal of the Alzheimer's Association. 2018;doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219 in press.

Stern Y., Gazes Y., Razlighi Q., Steffener J., Habeck C. A task-invariant cognitive reserve network. NeuroImage. 2018;178:36–45. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.033.

Stine-Morrow E.A.L. The Dumbledore hypothesis of cognitive aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007;16:300–304.

Stine-Morrow E.A.L., Milinder L., Pullara O., Herman B. Patterns of resource allocation in reading are reliable among younger and older readers. Psychology and Aging. 2001;16:69–84.

Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., & Miller, L. M. S. (2009). Aging, self-regulation, and learning from text. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), Vol. 51. Psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 255–296). New York: Elsevier.

Stine-Morrow E.A.L., Miller L.M.S., Hertzog C. Aging and self-regulated language processing. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132:582–606.

Sullivan E.V., Pfefferbaum A. Diffusion tensor imaging and aging. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2006;30:749–761.

Sullivan E.V., Rohlfing T., Pfefferbaum A. Quantitative fiber tracking of lateral and interhemispheric white matter systems in normal aging: Relations to timed performance. Neurobiology of Aging. 2010;31:464–481.

Tadayonnejad R., Ajilore O. Brain network dysfunction in late-life depression: A literature review. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology. 2013;27(1):5–12. doi:10.1177/0891988713516539.

Tapp P.D., Siwak C.T., Gao F.Q., Chiou J.Y., Black S.E., Head E., et al. Frontal lobe volume, function, and beta-amyloid pathology in a canine model of aging. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:8205–8213.

Taylor W.D., Aizenstein H.J., Alexopoulos G.S. The vascular depression hypothesis: Mechanisms linking vascular disease with depression. Molecular Psychiatry. 2013;18:963.

Teipel S., Meindl T., Wagner M., Kohl T., Bürger K., Reiser M.F., et al. White matter microstructure in relation to education in aging and Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Alzheimer's disease: JAD. 2009;Vol. 17:571–583.

Treadway M.T., Waskom M.L., Dillon D.G., Holmes A.J., Park M.T.M., Chakravarty M.M., et al. Illness progression, recent stress, and morphometry of hippocampal subfields and medial prefrontal cortex in major depression. Biological Psychiatry. 2015;77(3):285–294. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.06.018.

Tucker A.M., Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in aging. Current Alzheimer Research. 2011;8(4):354–360.

Turner R.J., Lloyd D.A. The stress process and the social distribution of depression. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1999;40:374–404.

Valenzuela M.J., Sachdev P. Brain reserve and dementia: A systematic review. Psychological Medicine. 2006;36(4):441–454. doi:10.1017/S0033291705006264.

Vallesi A., McIntosh A.R., Stuss D.T. Overrecruitment in the aging brain as a function of task demands: Evidence for a compensatory view. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;23:801–815.

Venezia R.G., Gorlyn M., Burke A.K., Oquendo M.A., Mann J.J., Keilp J.G. The impact of cognitive reserve on neurocognitive performance in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research. 2018;270:211–218. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2018.09.031.

Walhovd K.B., Westlye L.T., Amlien I., Espeseth T., Reinvang I., Raz N., et al. Consistent neuroanatomical age-related volume differences across multiple samples. Neurobiology of Aging. 2011;32:916–932.

Wang L., Laviolette P., O'Keefe K., Putcha D., Bakkour A., Van Dijk K.R.A., et al. Intrinsic connectivity between the hippocampus and posteromedial cortex predicts memory performance in cognitively intact older individuals. NeuroImage. 2010;51:910–917.

Wardlaw J.M., Hernández M.C.V., Muñoz-Maniega S. What are White Matter Hyperintensities Made of?. Journal of the American Heart Association. 2015;4:e001140.

Watson A., Joyce E. Cognitive reserve and neuropsychiatric disorders. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2015;4:142–146. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.05.003.

Williams J.M., Mathews A., MacLeod C. The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin. 1996;120(1):3–24.

Wlotko E.W., Lee C., Federmeier K.D. Language of the aging brain: Event-related potential studies of comprehension in older adults. Linguistics and Language Compass. 2010;4(8):623–638.

Yang A.C., Tsai S.J., Liu M.E., Huang C.C., Lin C.P. The association of aging with white matter integrity and functional connectivity hubs. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2016;8(2):919–1013.

Zahr N.M., Rohlfing T., Pfefferbaum A., Sullivan E.V. Problem solving, working memory, and motor correlates of association and commissural fiber bundles in normal aging: A quantitative fiber tracking study. NeuroImage. 2009;44:1050–1062.

Further reading

Bialystok E. Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2001.

Brune M., Brüne-Cohrs U. Theory of mind—Evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and psychopathology. Neuroscience & Biobehaioralv Reviews. 2006;30:437–455.

Carpenter P.A., Just M.A. The role of working memory in language comprehension. In: Klahr D., Kotovsky K., eds. Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1989:31–68.

Colangeli S., Boccia M., Verde P., Guariglia P., Bianchini F., Piccardi L. Cognitive reserve in healthy aging and Alzheimer's disease: A meta-analysis of fMRI studies. American Journal of Alzheimer's Disease and Other Dementias. 2016;31(5):443–449. doi:10.1177/1533317516653826.

Gernsbacher M.A., Faust M.E. The mechanism of suppression: A component of general comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1991;17:245–262.

Kroll J.F., Bobb S.C., Misra M., Guo T. Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychologica. 2008;128(3):416–430.

Kroll J.F., Sumutka B.M., Schwartz A.I. A cognitive view of the bilingual lexicon: Reading and speaking words in two languages. International Journal of Bilingualism. 2005;9(1):27–48.

Linck J.A., Kroll J.F., Sunderman G. Losing access to the native language while immersed in a second language: Evidence for the role of inhibition in second-language learning. Psychological Science. 2009;20(12):1507–1515.

Perfetti C.A. Reading ability: Lexical quality and comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2007;11:357–383.

Scarmeas N., Habeck C.G., Zarahn E., Anderson K.E., Park A., Hilton J., et al. Covariance PET patterns in early Alzheimer's disease and subjects with cognitive impairment but no dementia: Utility in group discrimination and correlations with functional performance. NeuroImage. 2004;23(1):35–45. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.032.

Sheline Y.I., Barch D.M., Garcia K., Gersing K., Pieper C., Welsh-Bohmer K., et al. Cognitive function in late life depression: Relationships to depression severity, cerebrovascular risk factors and processing speed. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;60(1):58–65. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.019.

..................Content has been hidden....................

You can't read the all page of ebook, please click here login for view all page.
Reset
3.138.175.180