Chapter 21
Drivers of Diversity in Consumers' Aesthetic Response to Product Design

Adèle Gruen

Introduction

When asking around what people think the main difference between art and product design is, one answer that you will often come across is that design is an activity with a purpose, and the purpose of a product is to be used. This basic idea has paved the way to the movement of user centered design, which invites product developers to reflect on who will be using their creation, how will they use it, for what purpose, when, and with whom. Designers are expected to think of how to create positive experiences between the user and the product, not just how to create beautiful products. User knowledge has thus become central in the process of product development. In the field of marketing research, academics have been studying consumers (i.e., product users) for more than a century. Looking at research developments in this discipline is interesting for product developers, as they will find theories, tools, and examples on the understanding of consumer behavior. Looking in the direction of marketing academic research can benefit not only designers, but also design managers, product developers, product managers, or anyone who wants to develop a product for a targeted consumer!

Design thinking theorists encourage new product developers to be concerned about and to focus on user experience. A great part of that experience depends on the aesthetics of the product. Aesthetic considerations therefore have their rightful place in design thinking, especially regarding user research. It is important to identify the variety of consumers and consumption contexts that can impact preferences overall, including aesthetic preferences. Consumer aesthetic preferences are strong determinants of future approach or avoidance behaviors toward the product (Bloch, 1995). That is why when analyzing feedback on a prototype, for instance, we understand how crucial it is to reflect on aesthetic preferences.

In this chapter we look at what marketing can tell us about the diversity of consumers' aesthetic responses. Aesthetics can be defined as “a sensitivity to the beautiful or a branch of philosophy that provides a theory of the beautiful and of the fine arts” (Veryzer, 1993). Aesthetic response refers to the reaction a person has to an object (e. g., product) based on his or her perception of the object (Berlyne, 1974). Veryzer (1993) goes further by stating that aesthetic response can be a reaction to conscious or unconscious aspects (i.e., stimulus, such as color, shape) of the product and that it leads to the registering of affect or pleasure (prior to buying the product, for instance).

Chapter 20 addressed consumer responses to product designs that are shared by most consumers. In this complementary chapter we will try to provide our readership with a (nonexhaustive) list of the drivers of differences in consumer's response, with a focus on aesthetic preferences. Why don't we all like the same designs? The chapter starts with the broad influence of culture (the culture of a country, the consumer culture, and the culture of class), before narrowing down the topic to individualities (personality, taste), and concludes with the importance of situational factors (Figure 21.1).

c21f001

Figure 21.1 Summary of the main influences on consumers' aesthetic preferences.

21.1 Culture

National and Regional Cultures

Culture is a lens through which people view a phenomenon, apprehend and assimilate it (McCracken, 1986). It has been said that beauty is partially in the eyes of the culture and not of the individual (Berlyne, 1971). Cultures influence how people see the world and impact how they visually appreciate the design of a product. Thus, the culture of a given individual is likely to significantly guide his or her aesthetic tastes.

Hofstede (1980) proposed a tool to classify cultures according to their values. This helps us to understand how cultures can systematically differ in terms of aesthetic preferences. Below, I present three cultural dimensions that vary across cultures and that may influence an individual's aesthetic response1 (Table 21.1):

  • Individualism/collectivism. Western countries are traditionally more individualistic cultures (North America and Europe), whereas Eastern and African countries tend to be more collectivistic.
  • Masculinity/femininity. Masculine cultures value competitiveness and materialism (Japan is one of the most masculine country). Feminine cultures value quality of life and happiness (the Nordic countries are known to be highly feminine countries in this respect).
  • Long-term orientation/short-term orientation. This dimension refers to the extent to which a country gives importance to the past and tradition. Countries that score high on this dimension will tend to have a pragmatic approach and encourage modernism. On the opposite countries that score low prefer to maintain traditions and norms.

Table 21.1 Visual Preferences and Cultural Dimensions

Dimension Example of Country Value Associated Visual Preferences
Collectivist China
Most of Asia and Africa
Social harmony
Interdependence within society members
Round Symmetric
Balanced
Individualistic United States Creativity
Independence of society members
New, innovative
Disruptive
Short-term oriented France Tradition, norms, Past Vintage, old-fashioned
Long-term oriented USA Modernism
Future
New
Innovative
Masculine Japan Materialism, competitiveness, dynamism Angular
Feminine Nordic countries Quality of life
Harmony, happiness
Rounded

In Japan and China, designs that foster social harmony will be appreciated more because they fit with the collectivist view of these countries. For instance, symmetric and balanced designs will be favored because these are factors for harmonious design (Henderson, Cote, Leong, & Schmitt, 2003). In individualistic countries such as the United States (which scores 100 out of 100 on this value!), creativity is highly valued. In this context, new, innovative designs will have a better chance of being accepted (Henderson et al., 2003; Schmitt & Simonson, 1997). Time orientation also can influence people's visual tastes. Countries that give a lot of importance to their history and that value their past, such as China, and also Europe (“the old continent”), will place higher value on traditional designs with long life expectancy (think of the longevity and success of the Channel No. 5 fragrance for instance).

Henderson et al. (2003) also found that American consumers tended to favor angular shapes, whereas their Asian counterparts favor rounded shapes. An explanation of this fact can be found in the work of Zhang, Feick, and Price (2006). Zhang et al. found that rounded versus angular shapes preferences could be linked to self-construal. More specifically, an independent self-construal (i.e., the self is defined as independent of others) is associated with confrontation, whereas an interdependent self-construal (i.e., the self is defined as interdependent with others) is associated with conflict avoidance. Independent individuals who value confrontation perceive angular shapes as more attractive and rounded shapes as less attractive than individuals with interdependent self-construal do. Angular shapes reflect conflict and dynamism and are also often associated with masculine cultures. Feminine cultures, however, are often believed to favor more rounded, soft shapes. When thinking about developing a product, the choice of angular shapes may receive less approval in a feminine culture that values harmony in life than in a masculine culture (Schmitt & Simonson, 1997).

These are just guidelines: what of a country such as Japan, which scores high on masculinity and low on individualism? According to Hofstede, masculinity prevails over collectivism in Japan, yet a careful look at the values and culture needs to be taken before launching a new product in a culture with seemingly contradictory values.

There are also differences among cultures in color associations. For instance, in Western society, which is more individualistic, white stands for purity and brides are mostly dressed in white. In Japan, white is the color used for mourning and grieving (Whitfield & Wiltshire, 1983).

Cultures influence design beyond visual aesthetics by affecting the role of a product. Across countries, different lifestyles impact the designing of products. Bike designs, for example, differ in the United States and in the Netherlands. In the United States, roads are wider and people are used to having cars to travel around. In the United States, bikes are leisure products, used with family or friends on the weekends to ride through the countryside. North American bikes need to be robust to respond to those outdoor activities. In the Netherlands, the roads are quite narrow. The average traveling distance is shorter, and people have developed the habit of traveling by bike, which is also quicker in case of traffic jams. In the Netherlands and most of the Nordic countries, urban residents use their bikes individually as a means of transport. Dutch bikes are lighter, more practical, that is, more adapted to city riding than North American ones. In France and in the United Kingdom, bikes need to answer to both activities at the same time, thus contributing to the success of hybrid bike designs such as the successful Decathlon's B'Twin of France (as shown in Figure 21.2).

c21f002

Figure 21.2 Example of bike designs in (a) the United States; (b) the Netherlands; and (c) France.

Sources: (a) Wikimedia Commons/Rbv123; (b) Wikimedia Commons/Vijverln; (c) Wikimedia Commons/Przemysław Jahr.

American bikes are traditionally designed for outdoor activities, whereas Dutch bikes must respond to urban transportation needs. In France and in the United Kingdom, where both activities are equally important, hybrid bikes are very successful (Figure 21.2).

Cultures cannot be reduced to national or regional boundaries. Not every North American consumer likes innovative product designs, nor does every African like traditional ones. Culture is a complex, multilevel phenomenon. In order to understand a consumer, one must grasp the many influences on which his identity is built, among which (but certainly not exhaustively) are social class and consumer culture.

The Concept of Class

The French sociologist Pierre Bourdieu established in his book Distinction: A Social Critique of the Judgment of Taste (1979, English trans. 1984) that thanks to the cultural capital we inherit our tastes are defined by the social class in which we are born. He found that abstract paintings were highly valued by the “petite bourgeoisie,” for instance, in order to differentiate their tastes from those of the working class. The working class favored pictures of everyday life such as a first communion or a folklore dance, which they understood and identified with. Though Bourdieu has had many critics, his analysis remains intriguing and relevant. A too visually complex, abstract product will probably not be appealing to the working class. However, individuals who wish to belong to the social elite will probably disregard trivial or very common products and favor distinguished ones. This phenomenon can explain the achievement of Nespresso®, the successful coffee machine and capsule manufacturer. The aesthetic of Nespresso is very peculiar, both product-wise (neat, elegant) and distribution-wise (flagship stores with a doorman). This specific design is associated with upper-class tastes and can be considered snobby by those who do not identify with it. Indeed, owning a Nespresso machine is a sign of social status in Europe today. Bourdieu has shown that taste has a lot to do with the social origins of an individual.

Holbrook (1999, 2005) studied taste and expertise and found a strong correlation between the two. People with expertise have the ability to define what is of good taste and what is not. Taste can be defined as: “An individual's consistent and appropriate response to aesthetic consumption objects through any of the five senses that is highly correlated with some external standard” (Hoyer & Stokburger-Sauer, 2012). The correlation between taste and external standards, defined by experts, helps us understand the work of Bourdieu (1984 [1984]). It is the upper class of society that has the “knowledge” and “expertise” in term of tastes. The lower class, with lower cultural background will, with time, transfer its tastes to the standards established by “experts.” However, the standard of beauty of the upper class will by that time already be elsewhere. Thus, there will always be a distinction in taste between people of different social class (Bourdieu, 1984 [1984]).

Subcultures of Consumption

The subculture of consumption is yet another frame affecting visual preferences. People who follow a specific type of consumption are likely to adopt its aesthetics standards (Schouten & McAlexander, 1995). This is the case for Goths, for instance, whose community has its own aesthetics in clothes, music, and way of life (Schilt, 2007). Individuals who identify with this particular culture of consumption will value products with design congruent to the community's standards. In the case of Goths, for instance, there is a preference for the color black for clothing, or dark objects with sharp metal to express dramatization of the self. When designing a store for that target, for instance, designers need to be intentional given that individuals who identify with the Gothic community might feel less at ease in a store playing loud pop music than they would in one playing Lou Reed's albums. Aesthetic standards are present in many subcultures or communities, such as the gay and lesbian community, bikers, rockers, punks, and hipsters, for example.

Culture is a multilevel, complex phenomenon. It is important to always ask yourself a few questions regarding your future user's culture before thinking about developing a product (see Figure 21.3).

c21f003

Figure 21.3 Nonexhaustive external influences of consumer's aesthetic preferences.

21.2 Individual Characteristics

Though the same influences are exerted in a given culture, people within that culture still can have completely opposite aesthetic tastes. Let's take the example of two young girls, Cate and Jenny. They are both 25, British, and living in London. They graduated from the same business school and are now dashing, young executives. On paper, a first glimpse at their situation and we could be tempted to “classify” them into the same segment, for instance, “cosmopolitan young adult.” Yet, when looking closer, we realize that Cate, who is an analyst in a major bank, comes from a British countryside bourgeois family, has her own flat, likes to go shopping in antique boutiques, and wears vintage clothing. Jenny works in a design consultancy firm. Her family is from London, and she lives with her mother, who is a fashion designer; she never misses any avant-garde exhibition, and makes her own clothes most of the time. Our perspective on their aesthetic preferences has changed. If the challenge is to design a desk for those young women, we might decide on something more traditional, wood-made for Cate, for instance, and perhaps something more modern for Jenny. But how can we know who our consumers are? If the analysis of culture helps new product developers to get a broad sense of the aesthetics standards and expectations of their targeted population, adding individual characteristics into the equation will allow a better understanding of who the consumer is and what he or she desires. For instance, the importance of design in one's life and personality are great influencers of one's aesthetic preferences.

Centrality of Visual Product Aesthetics

Bloch, Brunel, and Arnold (2003) postulated that individual's aesthetic preferences differed depending on the importance of a product's visual appearance in one's life. They named this characteristic the centrality of visual product aesthetics and developed a conceptual model and a scale to measure this variable. What the centrality of visual product aesthetics (CVPA) exactly refers to is “the overall level of significance that visual aesthetics hold for a particular consumer in his/her relationships with products” (Bloch et al., 2003). This concept can be explained through three subdimensions: value, acumen, and response (Figure 21.4).

  1. Value. This subdimension refers to the importance given by someone to a product's design in general. This is the extent to which people believe design has the ability to enhance “quality of life, both personally and for society in general” (Bloch et al., 2003). For example, do you agree that “beautiful product design make our world a better place”? This subdimension will measure the extent to which individuals believe that product appearance can improve their personal and the society's well-being. Designers, artists, and probably anyone with a sensibility for art are likely to score high on this dimension. In the example we used at the beginning, we can guess that both Cate and Jenny might score high on this dimension, as they are both careful in choosing their clothes and their furniture.
  2. Acumen. This subdimension assesses the ability to evaluate, to identify, and to categorize a product design. Some people are likely to have a higher aesthetic awareness than others. These individuals are expected give greater weight to design elements in product decision making. People who work in a highly aesthetic environment may show more design acumen than others. Jenny works in a fashion consultancy, is surrounded every day by highly aesthetic products and clothes; discussions around her are revolving around artistic or visual issue. We can guess that due to her working environment she will score higher than Cate on this dimension.
  3. Response. This dimension measures the level of aesthetic response to product designs. It consists of both psychological response (enjoyment, affect, etc.) and physical (behavioral) response (approach, willingness to buy, etc.) (Bloch, 1995). Good design is often said to provoke impulse buying. Some individuals will be more prone to act on their feelings toward a product. Those persons often engage more with their possessions and build strong relationships with them. They will not see a product as a mere “dead object” but will grant it a personal intrinsic life. This is perhaps more the case for Cate, who likes vintage clothes and furniture: she gives importance to the historical value and significance of the product.
c21f004

Figure 21.4 The making of CVPA.

The authors showed that high-CVPA individuals evaluate highly aesthetic products as more pleasing and, have a more positive attitude toward them and a higher purchase intention for these products. In the world of fashion, these individuals are more likely to be opinion leaders and fashion innovators than low-CVPA individuals, who most likely tend to be fashion followers (Workman & Caldwell, 2007). It is crucial to understand, when developing a product, if the targeted users have higher or lower centrality of visual product aesthetics. Low-CVPA individuals discriminate less strongly between aesthetically pleasing and not pleasing products. They presented no differences in behavioral and purchase intentions between low- and high-aesthetic products with same utility (Bloch et al., 2003). In short, people who have the ability to enjoy more complex designs and for whom aesthetics in life is important will be more difficult to please. Those who do not might look at other criteria as primary influencers such as price or function when buying a product.

Visualizing Tendency

Morris Holbrook (1986) argued for the importance of the role of personality variables in affecting aesthetic response. He identified personality variables that can promote variety in aesthetic judgments such as the tendency to process information visually or verbally, the tendency to be more romanticist or classicist, and gender. His research showed that as personalities differ so do aesthetic judgments. Visualizers, for instance, will give more importance to holistic patterns when processing information (Holbrook, 1986). Holbrook found that these persons were more represented among romanticists, which means that visualizers are more likely to enjoy complex designs than those with verbalizing processing tendencies. Bloch et al. (2003) confirmed this with a study showing higher scores for visualizers on the centrality of visual product aesthetics. Holbrook identified visualizers as also being more strongly represented amongst women. Those visualizing women, for instance, in terms of fashion choices, will prefer “plain jackets and (at most) one isolated or two nonadjacent set(s) of stripes” (Holbrook, 1986). Visualizers have a more holistic view of the outfit and thus give more importance to the combination of elements around the jacket. Verbalizing women, however, who do not have such a holistic view, will prefer “plaid jackets.” The difference between visualizers and verbalizers is less important for men; in the end they “play it safe by disliking clashing designs.” Finally, and not surprisingly, classicists might prefer products with more neat designs, while romanticists may prefer more stylish, ornate designs. These results are to be taken with care. It would be inaccurate to suggest that all men dislike clashing designs, for instance.

Need for Uniqueness and Optimal Stimulation Level

The need to distinguish oneself from others is a strong determinant of behavior toward objects (Snyder & Fromkin, 1980). The products we own are used every day in the slow construction of identity; they are bought, consumed, displayed, and destroyed in order to build who we are (Belk, 1988). For some individuals who feel a need to be unique, the product they buy will play a much important role. These individuals will put more care into choosing a product: “Is it unique enough for me?” Snyder and Fromkin (1980) showed that these individuals tend to prefer novel or unusual products that reflect their unique personalities. Not surprisingly, Bloch et al. (2003) found that people who score high on the CVPA scale are also the ones who will seek more unique products.

For the majority of people, however, the optimal level of stimulation (OSL) needed to feel arousal is moderate (Berlyne, 1960). For some people, however, this optimal level is greater; they need more stimulation than others to feel stimulated in their everyday lives. These individuals have been identified as “high optimum stimulation levels (OSL) individuals” (Steenkamp & Baumgartner, 1992). The authors found that these consumers will engage in more variety-seeking behavior as they get bored more quickly by a product. They also are more risk-seeking individuals. Raju (1980) studied the relationship between OSL and personality and demographic traits. High-OSL individuals are attracted to new or unusual stimuli or situation. They may, for instance, work in highly stimulating environments such as museums, theatres, and art schools. Drawing their attention requires greater visual stimuli and higher creativity in the designing of a product. In terms of demographics, younger, educated, and employed people seem to have a higher OSL (Raju, 1980). To decorate her home interior, Jenny, who works in a design consultancy, may prefer original and innovative furniture that will offer her psychological benefits such as risk, variety, or novelty.

21.3 Situational Factors

Situational factors are well known in consumer research to be crucial factors influencing behaviors (Belk, 1975). They play a moderating role in the relationship between product design and consumer response (Bloch, 1995), which means that depending on the situation, a person may not have the same response (aesthetic, behavioral) toward a product. We can distinguish between the physical context and the influence of others (social environment).

When talking about context, we mean the context in which the product will be displayed or consumed. The fit of the new product with products already owned by the individual might, for instance, influence its perception. Consumers may like a product's appearance but may not buy it because it does not fit aesthetically with their home interior (Bloch, 1995). If someone owns an old cottage in the countryside decorated with old furniture, he or she may decide not to choose a twenty-first-century-design sofa for the living room. The context can also change a product's function. For instance, many vintage products are used for aesthetic purposes and have lost their utilitarian function (Veryzer, 1995). It is common to find old sewing machines or vintage clocks that have stopped functioning in living rooms for decorating purposes.

The social context also greatly influences the aesthetic response of a consumer to a product. Being alone or being with others will change the way consumers behave toward a product (Belk, 1975). The work of Zhang et al. (2006) offers a great example of that fact. They found that independent persons tended to favor angular shapes. However, that relationship was significant only when in the presence of others. In society, we try to give an image of a confident self, perhaps more confident than in reality. Showing a preference for angular shapes may be believed to reflect a sharp personality. Indeed, angular shapes are associated with masculine features, as we have seen previously.

It is important to reflect on what the product will be used for, where it will be used, and with whom. Will this chair be for the bedroom, where no one except family goes? Or for the living room, which is the place where we can show others who we are (by what we own)?

21.4 Discussion

This chapter provides those who wish to develop new products with a list of factors to pay attention to. This list is not exhaustive. My goal is to encourage product developers to consider the many influences of their consumers' aesthetic preferences. I particularly focused on the influence of cultures, individual characteristics, and situation.

First, we saw the crucial role of culture. Culture acts as a lens through which we see the world (McCracken, 1986), therefore modifying the way consumers approach new products. Chinese consumers may be more sensitive to rounded shapes due to the collectivist nature of their culture. This has an impact on logo design. In China and Singapore it has been found that brand logos that are harmonious and natural create more positive affect towards the brand (Henderson et al., 2003). However, American consumers will probably value innovative design more. Culture can also modify the role of a product as we have seen with bikes, which are used for leisure in North American and for transport in northern Europe. Culture is not only a question of geography, and we have seen that social classes have their own aesthetic standards. When a consumer is involved in a specific consumption activity, it impacts his entire life, including aesthetic preferences like in the case of Goths or bikers.

Second, we looked at personality traits and individual characteristics that may impact the way consumers react to a product. We have seen that for some consumer aesthetics plays a big part in life; they will be more attentive to product design and more demanding as well. People who have greater need to feel unique than average will focus their choices on differentiating products. Others need greater visual stimulation than average to feel stimulated and thus will favor more flashy colors or more unique designs. Even though it is difficult to segment a population according to such criteria, some clues can be taken into account. Holbrook (1986), for instance, found that there were more visualizers among women. Raju (1980) found that young, educated, and employed people were more likely to need novel, varied, and innovative products. The working environment may be a way of knowing if our consumers give greater importance to aesthetics in their lives or if they need more stimulation. People who work in the art world might be more demanding, for instance. The question of whom we are designing the product for is crucial. If the wish is to make a product that will appeal to a broad audience, it is worth noting that a medium level of stimulation will be preferable: too little and the product might not be noticed by consumers; too much and it might be rejected (Berlyne, 1960; Steenkamp & Baumgartner, 1992). However, if the strategy is to target opinion leaders to create an image for the brand or the product, a highly stimulating design with innovative features might a good choice (Workman & Caldwell, 2007).

Finally, it is crucial to look out for the consumption context of the future product. Depending on where and with whom we consume, the product may not have the same appeal. We invite product developers to reflect on the situation in which their consumers will buy, use, display, and consume their product.

21.5 Conclusion

This chapter provides the reader with a nonexhaustive list of forces that sway the aesthetics preferences of consumers. This list is to be used carefully: in the topic of aesthetics there is nothing ever certain and definite. Fashion progresses at a rapid pace and tastes change, evolve, and mutate fast. Also, there are many more factors not mentioned here that consumers consider before choosing a product, like price or functionality. We encourage anyone who wishes to launch a new product to engage into a reflective activity around the targeted consumer. Who do we want our consumers to be, and what do we know about their aesthetic preferences or about the forces that will influence their choices? These are things to consider before thinking of the design itself.

References

  1. Belk, R. W. (1975). Situational variables and consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 2(3), 157–164.
  2. Belk, R. W. (1988). Possessions and the extended self. Journal of Consumer Research, 15(2), 139–168.
  3. Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
  4. Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  5. Berlyne, D. E. (1974). Studies in the new experimental aesthetics: Steps toward an objective psychology of aesthetic appreciation. Oxford, England: Hemisphere.
  6. Bloch, P. H. (1995). Seeking the ideal form: Product design and consumer response. Journal of Marketing, 59, 16–29.
  7. Bloch, P. H., Brunel, F. F., & Arnold, T. J. (2003). “Individual differences in the centrality of visual product aesthetics: concept and measurement.” Journal of Consumer Research, 29, 551–565.
  8. Bourdieu, P. (1979; English trans. 1984). Distinction: A social critique of the judgment of taste. Boston, MA: Harvard University Press.
  9. Henderson, P. W., Cote, J. A., Leong, S. M., & Schmitt, B. (2003). Building strong brands in Asia: Selecting the visual components of image to maximize brand strength. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 20(4), 297–313.
  10. Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture and organizations. International Studies of Management & Organization, 10(4), 15–41.
  11. Holbrook, M. B. (1986). Aims, concepts, and methods for the representation of individual differences in aesthetic responses to design features. Journal of Consumer Research, 13(3), 337–347.
  12. Holbrook, M. B. (1999). Popular appeal versus expert judgments of motion pictures. Journal of Consumer Research, 26(2), 144–155.
  13. Holbrook, M. B. (2005). The role of ordinary evaluations in the market for popular culture: Do consumers have “good taste”? Marketing Letters, 16(2), 75–86.
  14. Hoyer, W. D., & Stokburger-Sauer, N. E. (2012). The role of aesthetic taste in consumer behavior. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 40(1), 167–180.
  15. McCracken, G. (1986). Culture and consumption: A theoretical account of the structure and movement of the cultural meaning of consumer goods. Journal of Consumer Research, 13(1), 71–84.
  16. Raju, P. S. (1980). Optimum stimulation level: its relationship to personality, demographics, and exploratory behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 7(3), 272–282.
  17. Schilt, K. (2007). Queen of the Damned : Women and Girls' Participation in Two Gothic Subcultures, In L. M. Goodlad & M. Bibby (Eds.), Goth: Undead subculture. Durham, NC: Duke University Press
  18. Schmitt, B. H., & Simonson, A. (1997). Marketing aesthetics. New York, NY: Free Press.
  19. Schouten, J. W., & McAlexander, J. H. (1995). Subcultures of consumption: An ethnography of the new bikers. Journal of Consumer Research, 22(1), 43–61.
  20. Snyder, C. R., & Fromkin, H. L. (1980). Uniqueness: The human pursuit of difference. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
  21. Steenkamp, J. B. E., & Baumgartner, H. (1992). The role of optimum stimulation level in exploratory consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 19(3), 434–448.
  22. Veryzer, R. W. (1993). Aesthetic response and the influence of design principles on product preferences. Advances in Consumer Research, 20(1), 224–228.
  23. Veryzer, R. W. (1995). The place of product design and aesthetics in consumer research. In F. R. Kardes & M. Sujan (Eds.), Advances in consumer research (pp. 641–645). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research.
  24. Whitfield, A., & Wiltshire, T. (1983). Color. In C. H. Flurscheim (Ed.), Industrial design in engineering (pp. 133–157). Worcester, England: The Design Council.
  25. Workman, J. E., & Caldwell, L. F. (2007). Centrality of visual product aesthetics, tactile and uniqueness needs of fashion consumers. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 31(6), 589–596.
  26. Zhang, Y., Feick, L., & Price, L. J. (2006). The impact of self-construal on aesthetic preference for angular versus rounded shapes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(6), 794–805.

About the Author

AdÈle Gruen is a PhD student at the Université-Paris Dauphine in Paris, France. Her doctoral dissertation focuses on the role of design in the appropriation of objects in the context of collaborative consumption. This present work is based on her thesis for the master of research and research methods she attended at that same university.

..................Content has been hidden....................

You can't read the all page of ebook, please click here login for view all page.
Reset
3.142.12.207