CHAPTER 7
Bedrock—Psychology
In 1920, John Watson (1878-1958) conducted an experiment in his laboratory at Johns Hopkins University. He placed a rabbit next to a 9-month-old boy named Albert, who immediately was attracted and reached out to the furry animal. Then, out of sight of the boy, Watson made a very loud sound by hitting a steel bar with a hammer.
Innumerable introductory psychology students have witnessed the heart-wrenching film of what happens next: “Little Albert” jerks back suddenly at the loud sound and begins to cry. After seven repetitions, the boy began to cry as soon as the rabbit was presented to him, even without the sound. Little Albert had been conditioned to fear rabbits. Watson was engaged in establishing behaviorism as a major theory to explain human behavior, and it all had to do with observable and objectively measurable behavior, exactly what the science of the day required. Watson had introduced behaviorism to North American psychology.
Psychology is a modern attempt to apply scientific methods to questions that have interested philosophers for centuries:
• Can we predict what other people will do?
• Can we influence how they behave?
• Are there “natural laws” that underlie our actions?
• How can we observe and measure behavior so as to discover those laws?
• Is there a role for subjective experience in studying people’s actions?
• How much can we understand about an individual divorced from context?
• What are personalities and how do we get them?
• How can we explain the similarities and differences among people?
Psychology finds its roots in the ancient philosophers we explored in chapter 2. Understanding the “psyche,” the Greek word for “soul” or “spirit,” was an inseparable part of religious, spiritual, and health teachings throughout most of human history, but the physical and mental were separated in Western thought after medieval times.
HISTORICAL INTERLUDE
The separation between physical and spiritual (including mind) began to be challenged in the 16th century when the name “psychology”—putting together “psyche” or “soul” and the Greek word for study or knowledge—was first used (Green, 2007). The newly named psychology became a rich topic for philosophers over the next three centuries, but scientists mainly busied themselves discovering laws of the physical world, leaving the “soul” or “psyche” to the church and philosophers.
The increasing secularization of the 19th century allowed science to bring its methods to bear on the psyche. The “scientizing” of psychology can be traced to 1879, when Wilhelm Wundt (1832-1920) established the first psychology laboratory at Germany’s University of Leipzig. In keeping with scientific procedures of the time, Wundt carefully measured human abilities and perception, such as the perceived heaviness of different levels of weight, and called his studies “psychophysics.” Wundt’s laboratory promised to apply the same principles of objectivity to psychological phenomena that had led to the advances of industrialization. Thus, it attracted scholars from all over the world, who then went back to their home countries to introduce the work to others. One of these was the American philosopher William James (1842-1910).
James founded the first psychology laboratory in the United States at Harvard University. He followed Wundt’s model in the laboratory, but James retained an interest in topics beyond what could be measured in laboratories of the day. James is best known for his work The Principles of Psychology (1950), a huge two-volume encyclopedia of psychological topics of interest in his era. These included brain function, stream of thought, the self, habit, perception, attention, voluntary movement, and the will.
James’s approach to psychology, now over 100 years old, has remarkable parallels to coaching and eventually to neuroscience and quantum theory.
At the beginning of the 20th century in North America, psychology stood at a crossroads between the subjective, introspective exploration of “unseen mental processes” typified by James and the emphasis on measurement that characterized Wundt’s experimental approach. Wundt himself engaged in introspective inquiry, but it was the experiments conducted in his laboratory that fit best with an attempt to conform to “hard” science. Careful measurement and experimentation had met with enormous success in providing technological solutions for manufacture and transportation, and it therefore enjoyed great prestige. Why not apply those methods to the psyche?
As the 19th century changed to the 20th century, problems of business, education, government, and the military were presenting challenges for the emerging dominance of the United States. As psychological critic Isaac Prilleltensky (1994) wrote, psychology “did not hesitate to volunteer its services in exchange for recognition as the ‘master’ science of human affairs” (p. 28).
Psychology was successful in negotiating this exchange. It fulfilled its part of the bargain by assuming the mantle of value-free seeker of objective truth. According to Prilleltensky (1994), “By portraying itself as a strictly ‘objective’ endeavor, psychology erroneously interprets many of its prescriptive biases as merely descriptive assertions about human behavior” (p. 25, emphases in original).
Psychology also adopted a pillar of Western capitalist ideology: that self-interest is the primary route for the promotion of well-being. In this view, “the self is conceived of as a supreme entity with magnificent powers” (Prilleltensky, 1994, p. 17). Once it is assumed that the self supersedes the system, change can come only from the self, and not the organization or society. This assumption relates not only to understanding behavior but also to how we go about changing it. The shift to recognizing social context as a crucial aspect of maintaining and creating change is a theme of great relevance to coaching. It appears in nearly every chapter of this book.
But the beginning of the 20th century was the heyday of mechanistic science. In taking on the mantle of science, psychology rejected the subjective approach of James, the holistic perspective of classical healers, the dialectic and historical traditions of Hegel and Marx, and the willingness to admit and examine underlying values. These “systemic” elements lay fallow, to be rediscovered in North America at the very time when coaching emerged. How psychology lost and then began to recover these elements is the story that underlies the topics in this chapter.
Psychology is divided into a number of subdisciplines, most of which have applications to various areas of human or social endeavor, and many of which have direct relevance to coaching. Because of the breadth of psychological inquiry, other subdisciplines will be examined in subsequent chapters. These four bedrock areas and a summary will be presented in order to discover their usefulness in a coaching context:
• Behaviorism
• Psychometrics
• Developmental psychology
• Evolutionary psychology
• Psychology as bedrock for coaching

BEHAVIORISM

The subjective effect of James Watson’s experiment with Little Albert appeared to be of no concern to the experimenters. It is not known what happened to the boy as a result of this experiment, and it should be noted that treating people as experimental subjects without considering possible ill effects is no longer permitted in psychology laboratories. Strict ethical guidelines regarding the use of human beings in psychological and other research are now in place (see American Psychological Association website).
However, in his time, Watson was in a line of eminent researchers including the famous Ivan Pavlov (1849-1936) in Russia. Pavlov conditioned dogs to salivate to a bell after it had been rung time and again just before the introduction of meat powder into the dogs’ mouths. Pavlov’s careful documentation and Wundt’s measurement of human sensory abilities combined with Watson’s experiments, such as those with Little Albert, to make it seem possible to develop a science of mind that was as objective and rigorous as any of the natural sciences.
HISTORICAL INTERLUDE
It was not long before applications of the new behaviorist science spread outside the laboratory. In the very year of the Little Albert experiment, Watson’s affair with his lab assistant was discovered, and he was asked to resign from his professorship at Johns Hopkins.
After divorcing his wife, Watson married his former lab assistant and took a job at the V. Walter Thompson advertising agency. Promoted to vice president in 1924, he put to work the psychological principles he had developed in the laboratory, selling such products as Pond’s Cold Cream, Maxwell House coffee, and Johnson’s Baby Powder.
Many of the techniques that advertisers use to this day were developed during Watson’s years in advertising. For example, he translated the Little Albert experiment into a positive frame. If you show or tell us over and over about something we react to positively (a sexy model, desirable clothes, delicious food), then follow that immediately with a product, consumers will “associate” that positive reaction with the product and buy more of it. We may complain about the incessant repetition of ads on TV, the radio, the Internet, or billboards, but early “scientific” research showed repetition to be effective. If variety was proved to be the key to sales, you can bet we would never see the same ad twice.
The most influential figure in the history of behaviorism is B. F. Skinner (1904-1990). Reading a description of Watson’s behaviorist philosophy changed his life. As a doctoral student in psychology at Harvard University, he invented equipment (known popularly as the Skinner Box) to time and measure reinforcement and responses. As a professor at several universities and then back at Harvard, Skinner developed the theory of operant conditioning: We behave the way we do because of certain consequences we have experienced. Skinner denied that feelings or intentions play any part in determining behavior. These were “unseen mental processes” that occurred in the “black box” of the mind, not directly observable (at least not back then) and therefore not admissible as scientific data. In the spirit of the mechanistic paradigm, only the conditions, termed “stimuli,” and consequences of behavior, termed “reinforcers,” were of interest.
Skinner claimed to have found a new version of psychology, which he called behavior analysis, based on his philosophy of radical behaviorism. According to this approach, human actions are always really reactions that can be studied in a laboratory the same as reactions of any animal might be studied. Behaviorists equated children with the rats, monkeys, fish, cats, and chickens on which they experimented, and then applied learning from these experiments to the classroom, to the training of teachers, to military training, and thus to the world. People were assumed to have been born as “blank slates.” Obtaining whatever behavior one might desire from them was a matter of arranging proper conditions and consequences. Steven Pinker (2002) more recently argues vigorously against this approach, but in the 1950s, introductory psychology courses primarily focused on behaviorism as the core of scientific psychology.
The impact of behaviorist thought on our daily lives cannot be underestimated. Watson wrote articles for popular magazines of the time, insisting, for example, that parents who show their children too much affection run the risk of ruining their child’s healthy development. He contributed to the development of the self-help movement, one that continues to be guided by whatever psychological or pseudoscientific finding is popular at the moment. Skinner did not hesitate to apply the results of his laboratory research to human life and society in general. The government of the United States used “scientific” principles to sell the public on entering World War I (further discussed in chapter 13 in the section on social psychology). Hitler and the Nazis did the same to garner support for their war efforts. Psychological research has been used to enhance police and military techniques of intimidation and torture and allegedly even political and economic control (Klein, 2007).
The enormous influence of behaviorism on psychotherapy is covered in part IV. While some applications of behaviorism are reprehensible, the field has also left a legacy that has contributed to society in many ways. Behaviorism was behind the growth of the animal research field, which led to a better understanding of issues such as our relationship to drug dependence, motivational systems, learning processes, and how we cope with stress, not to mention a number of neuroscience discoveries.
In terms of research, behaviorist literature is enormous. Some of the specific principles that have emerged include:
• The law of exercise: repetition strengthens learning.
• The law of effect: the effect of reward is to strengthen learning.
• The principle of fast feedback: the optimum time between response and reinforcement is about half a second. In other words, for positive feedback to have an impact, it should be immediate.
• Punishment tends to be less effective than positive reinforcement combined with no reinforcement. Punishment may have unintended consequences and, rather than inhibiting the targeted behavior, often results in continuing the behavior when the punisher is not present.
Behavioral psychology’s impact on coaching is also substantial. From this field, we learned that people can change and that we—and they—can arrange consequences to impact that change.
For example, Skinner introduced a three-stage behavior training method:
1. Define the goal or terminal behavior desired.
2. Define the starting point (the current behavior).
3. Positively reinforce each step in the desired direction while ignoring all other behavior.
Example: “Train the Professor”
University students prove the efficacy of these steps every time they pull the “train the professor” prank. Before a class, students decide on a goal (terminal behavior). For example, they may decide to have the professor stand close to the left side of the chalkboard. The professor begins the class, say, behind the lectern on the right of the board (current behavior). Students look down and away—whatever might indicate a lack of interest or attention (ignoring behavior that does not lead toward the goal). Whenever the professor makes a move toward the left of the chalkboard, the students sit up and pay attention (positively reinforcing goal-related behavior). After many repetitions, to the delight of budding behavioral scientists in the room, the professor is standing close to and on the left of the chalkboard.
Dog owners and parents will recognize these steps. The fact that attention, treats, or praise work only when the professor, dog, or child is “hungry” for that particular reinforcement brings up cognitive questions of expectation, motivation, personality, and context. For reasons such as that, both coaching and neuroscience have moved beyond behaviorism while retaining its useful elements.
Coaches can help clients develop behavioral methods to manage and improve their results. For example, if we break a large project into smaller steps and then reward ourselves for completing each step, or if we allow ourselves dessert when we exercise three times a week, we are engaging the principles of operant conditioning. Behaviorism has a reputation of being manipulative, but when a client is involved in the development of an action plan, it becomes behavioral self-control rather than manipulation.
The behaviorist approach also contributed significantly to the development of “competencies” to capture observable performance standards. Professional regulatory or licensing bodies, such as for accounting, management, counseling, and coaching, have attempted to specify competencies (see, for example, the International Coach Federation website in Internet Links). Organizations identify specific behaviors that employees should be exhibiting in their various roles. Leadership competencies, for example, are now common in many organizations, with behavioral coaching made available to executives to help them improve competencies identified as less than optimal.
One of the proponents of “behavioral coaching” is Marshall Goldsmith (2003), an organizational coach who begins a coaching engagement by establishing an agreement with the client and his or her manager. The agreement defines the behaviors that are seen to be most likely to improve leadership effectiveness (establishing a goal or terminal behavior). A further agreement sets out who in the organization will determine whether the change has occurred after a year of step-by-step work with Goldsmith (comparing current behavior with desired future goal).
Overall, the behavioral approach is very much alive and well in organizational settings. When business leaders want to change the behaviors of large numbers of people, they often look first to behaviorism. Most managers swear by the principle of rewarding good behavior (e.g., with bonuses) in order to get more of it. This is the basis of the field of performance management. Managers also tend to believe in some form of punishment for behaviors that are not desired, though their options for delivery of punishment are increasingly limited by legal restrictions, union contracts, and an emphasis on promoting respectful relationships that has helped to fuel the popularity of coaching. How management is moving beyond this paradigm to a model that relies on “leadership” is covered in part V.
Even before technology provided the means for neuroscientists to observe events inside the black box of the brain, cognitive psychology had broken through the behaviorists’ objections to the scientific investigation of thought, emotion, meaning, and intentions. More on that in chapter 8. Meanwhile, it is important for coaches to realize that there are aspects of behaviorist discoveries that are difficult to ignore and that form part of the bedrock for both coaching and neuroscience.

Linking Behaviorism to Coaching

The overall behaviorist philosophy is something that sits well with some coaches and not with others. In summary, these three principles may be useful to coaches:
1. Provide carefully timed carrots and do not ignore the stick.
2. Either positive rewards or punishment accounts for the motivation to change.
3. Change comes from identifying a goal as defined in behavioral terms, building a step-by-step action plan, and providing reinforcement as motivation.

PSYCHOMETRICS

HISTORICAL INTERLUDE
The subdiscipline of psychological testing, or psychometrics, arose very early in the history of scientific psychology. In the 1890s at the University of Pennsylvania, if you were an experimental psychology student of Professor James Cattell, himself a student of Wundt, you would have taken a series of tests to measure the strength of your grip, how fast you could move your arm, how quickly you reacted to sound, and other simple functions. Cattell hoped to find easily measurable abilities (none involving any complex thinking skills) that would correlate with your grades and would be an indication of “intelligence.” He and colleagues at other universities of the time failed to find any such correlations, and the testing movement in North America stalled for a while.
But in France, Alfred Binet (1875-1911) was closely observing the intellectual development of his two daughters, and he concluded that complex mental processes such as abstract reasoning, not simple physiological abilities, were what marked intelligence. At the same time, Theodore Simon (1873-1961), also in France, was working with children who were then described as “retarded,” and he and other educators and psychologists such as Binet convinced the French government that educational opportunities for such children needed to be improved. But how to identify these children? Simon proposed to Binet a testing program based on the theories Binet had developed.
Based on trial and error with both “normal” and “retarded” children, the two men devised a test that in its final version in 1911 included five tasks for each year of childhood from age 3 to 12. Children who could perform all tasks up to and including the ones for their actual age would have a “mental age” that was the same as their chronological age.
For example, a child of 4 can typically copy a square but not a diamond. If a 4-year-old were able to copy a diamond (as normally only a 7-year-old can do), the chronological 4-year-old would have a mental age of 7, according to the original Binet test. In order to have measures that could be easily compared, psychometrists developed a quotient by dividing the mental age by the chronological age and then multiplying the result by 100. Thus, our 4-year-old diamond copier would have an intelligence quotient of 175—a genius indeed.
The intelligence quotient of a 7-year-old who could at best perform tasks typical of a 4-year-old would be 57, indicating eligibility for Simon’s special educational program. When word of the success of the Binet-Simon test reached the United States, a resurgence of interest in psychometrics resulted.
Assessment for such things as “intelligence” did not strictly fit the behaviorist paradigm, as the traits being tested were often “unseen mental processes.” However, because psychometrists dealt with measurement and statistics, many saw this as qualifying what they were doing as “real” science. Thus, psychometrics lent its weight to the growing acceptance among policy makers and social institutions of psychology as a science:
• Business wanted to know how to select workers who fit their jobs and expectations.
• Public education policy makers wondered how to select students for different types of educational opportunities. Lewis M. Terman (1877-1956) translated Binet’s work into English in 1916, extended it to age 15 and to adults, and developed what would become the Stanford-Binet intelligence test. Others followed, the most influential being the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) developed by David Wechsler (1896-1981) and still in use today, primarily in clinical and school psychology practice.
• The U.S. military faced the problem of sorting huge numbers of draftees and enlistees into World War I-related duties. Binet-type tests were difficult to administer to large numbers of subjects, so Robert Yerkes (1876-1956), who was president of the American Psychological Association at the time, headed a team that devised two paper-and-pencil tests: Army Alpha for those who could read and Army Beta for those who could not. The involvement of nearly 2 million men in this testing program supported the acceptance of psychometrics and the legitimacy of scientific psychology.
• The Thematic Apperception Test (TAT), a personality assessment developed by Henry A. Murray (1893-1988), was also used during the World War II efforts (OSS Assessment Staff, 1948) for selecting and assigning duties to soldiers. The TAT was an exception to behaviorist trends and utilized Murray’s long-standing relationship with Carl Jung for theoretical underpinnings.
• Psychiatrists wondered how to diagnose pathology in people who seemed to function well most or some of the time, so Starke R. Hathaway and J. C. Mckinley at the University of Minnesota hospitals developed the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) in 1942. Current versions are still widely in use. Administration and interpretation of the MMPI are restricted to trained physicians and clinical psychologists, and its emphasis on psychopathology makes it of little relevance to coaches.
• Less clinically oriented personality tests have proliferated as self-help and self-awareness movements stimulated widespread curiosity about one’s “personality.” Many of these assessments are used by coaches to provide a framework for thinking about a client’s approach to problem-solving, relationships, work, stress, and just about anything else in life.
Psychological tests are used extensively in nearly every other subdiscipline of psychology, and assessments have been developed to identify almost every conceivable trait or skill or attitude or potential. The computer has made it possible to conduct much more complex statistical analyses than were possible in the early attempts to determine school achievement, job fit, or military readiness. However “scientific” or “statistically valid” a test is, the question our clients ask is “What does it mean for me?”
In chapter 1, we referred to Clyde Kluckhohn’s quote:
 
All people are the same.
Some people are the same.
 
No people are the same. (Kluckhohn & Murray, 1948, p. 35)
 
 
These statements yield three different frameworks for thinking about and dealing with people. When comparative psychologists and linguists ask about how human empathy or language ability compares with that of apes or other animals, they are engaging the first framework and focusing on ways in which all people are the same.
Typically, psychometrists are interested in the second of Kluckhohn’s frameworks: the ways in which some people are the same. They devise measures that put people into categories based on the similarity of their responses.
The process of one-on-one coaching utilizes the third framework. A coaching engagement is, after all, a relationship between unique, one-of-a-kind individuals, and effective team or organizational coaching also honors this principle. Using “objective” assessments to ignore the uniqueness of clients (i.e., treating them as test scores or labels) is destructive of the respect and intimacy that is at the core of coaching. It may be useful to conceive of coaching about assessments as an opportunity to translate from the second to the third of Kluckhohn’s frameworks: to explore how assessment scores or categories are revealed or modified in this person’s unique life and context.
Many coaches consider it important to become trained in administering, or at least interpreting, the assessment instruments that are most often used in their context.
Whatever the specific assessment instrument that a coach uses or that a client brings to us for interpretation, certain principles apply:
• Knowing the category that describes how “some people are the same” can be very useful.
Example: “Slow and Deliberate”
Marilyn was required to complete an assessment as part of deciding on a career. She was categorized as being “slow and deliberate” rather than “quick and impetuous.” Her career counselor knew better than to suggest a job that required speed and suggested instead a position in which accuracy was valued.
• Treating the results of assessments as the final word on a person can lead to two errors: (1) the category (in Marilyn’s case, speed) is taken out of the person’s full social and historical context; and (2) the person may be treated as if she or he is the category.
Example: From Slow to Careful
When Marilyn brought the assessment to her coach because she had been asked in a performance review to improve the speed of her work, the coach shifted the framework to one that avoids these two potential errors. Rather than taking the category of “slow and deliberate” as being true at all times in all circumstances, the coach wondered when it was that Marilyn completed tasks more quickly. The coach discovered that anxiety caused Marilyn to slow down and check her work over and over. When Marilyn identified those circumstances in which she felt anxious, she was able to take steps to reduce their effects.
In addition, the coach wondered what meaning the speed of her work had for Marilyn—not for anyone else, not even for someone who scored exactly the same on the assessment. This framework stresses the individual uniqueness of each person—how “no people are the same.” Marilyn discovered the “slowness” that might have meant a lack of motivation for another employee actually was a sign of her commitment to her company and to her job. She wanted to make sure that she did her work exactly right.
When she reframed her “slowness” as “engagement,” Marilyn was able to negotiate new expectations with her boss. Even for her own self-image, she was able to think of herself as a person who sometimes did things slowly and carefully when she was unsure and when the outcome was very important. Marilyn felt very differently about this, as compared to thinking of herself as being slow.
• Many times clients bring assessments to coaches not because they misunderstand the content of interpretations they have been given but because they want to be treated as whole, unique human beings in exploring those interpretations.
• It is necessary to keep in mind what it is that the assessment is measuring. For example, if a test shows that a person is anxious, is that because the setting in which the test was administered is anxiety-producing? That is, if your job depends on how you do on a test, it would not be surprising to find that your state of anxiety goes up when you take it. That is different from an enduring trait of anxiety that a person might demonstrate whatever the setting.
• Another problem occurs when an assessment is seen as the personality or pattern or trait itself. The assessment is merely an indicator, a finger pointing to a personality or pattern or trait, not the actual trait. This is a variation of “the map is not the territory.” Thus, feedback from the coach must always be verified by the client if the results of an assessment are to be truly useful.
• Finally, all psychometric assessments are based on averages or other statistics about groups of people (some people who score the same). The tests indicate only probabilities regarding any individual person.

Linking Psychometrics to Coaching

The development of psychometric tests is a highly technical field far beyond the scope of this book. However, we do not have to be experts to understand that a particular client’s test scores are only as good as the quality of the assessment instrument itself. We see assessments in popular magazines—like “Four Questions to Tell if You’re a Really Cool Person.” Any of us can make up an assessment. We can assess the popularity of television shows by asking a dozen of our friends which ones they like. But then we may be surprised when our favorite show gets canceled. The assessments that give clients the most useful information are ones that were developed:
• By analyzing (“norming”) responses from a large number of different people (not just our friends, who tend to be like us).
• By making sure they measure what they say they measure (“validity”).
• By ensuring they come up with essentially the same results from test to test or over time (“reliability”).
The development and distribution of assessments is an industry in itself. Coaches who consider administering or interpreting a particular test are fortunate to have the resources of the Internet as an aid to due diligence. Many assessments and their scoring are also available on the Internet. Courses on how to deliver and interpret testing instruments abound. But we should never hesitate to ask test distributors for a plain-language explanation of how the assessment was developed and where and how others have used it.
In summary, coaches would do well to remember that:
• Formal, written assessments are simply different lenses for gathering information about people.
• People are so complex that their full richness cannot be conveyed by a single perspective, no matter how carefully designed the assessment instrument may be.
• “Objective” assessments and subjective conversations with clients can serve as two poles between which an increasingly rich understanding of our clients can cycle and emerge.

DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY

As universal public education became policy during the 19th century, governments sought ways to determine whether schooling was effective. To do this, they needed to understand what could reasonably be expected of children at different ages. Psychology was called on to provide the answer, and the field of developmental psychology was born.
HISTORICAL INTERLUDE
At the time in North America when behaviorism insisted on ignoring unseen mental processes, Jean Piaget (1896-1980) in Switzerland was making enormous strides in understanding how those processes develop. Early in his career, Piaget taught at the school in France run by Alfred Binet, the developer of intelligence testing introduced earlier in the section on psychometrics. Piaget noticed that students of a certain age tended to make the same mistakes in answering questions on the test. He was fascinated by the pattern of these errors and began to wonder if that and similar patterns characterized cognitive capacities at different ages. He and his wife, Valentine, had three children whom he studied from infancy. As a result of these observations, Piaget (1928) organized cognitive development into a series of stages—levels of development corresponding to infancy, childhood, and adolescence.
In Piaget’s scheme, from birth to age 2 is called the Sensorimotor Stage. From age 2 to age 7 is the Preoperational Stage, when motor skills are acquired. The Concrete Operational Stage occurs from age 7 to age 11, when children become able to think logically about concrete events. After age 11, they go through what Piaget termed the Formal Operational Stage, when they develop abstract reasoning. These stages have proven to be extremely important in education as well as in psychological development studies.
Piaget also developed a theory of “schemas,” another name for mental maps. He defined schemas as building blocks of simple ideas that we from into more complex ones. He proposed that in childhood we learn through “assimilation,” meaning that we take in information from our surroundings to form new schemas. As adults we learn through “accommodation,” which means using new information to change existing knowledge or schemas.
Piaget can be seen as contributing to constructivism in psychology, a stream of thought that developed outside of behaviorism and came into its own with the cognitive revolution to be discussed in chapter 8 in the section on cognitive psychology. Constructivist ideas have roots in the philosophy of Hans Vaihinger (1921) and in the sociological theory of Peter Berger and Thomas Luckmann (1966). Piaget influenced Lev Vygotsky (1896-1934), a Russian researcher whose ideas became known in the West in the 1960s; and Lawrence Kohlberg (1927-1987), who focused on the moral development of children and adolescents.
Developmental psychology studies age-related changes in behavior across the life span. Whereas behaviorists claimed that “we are whatever our conditioning makes us,” developmental psychologists said that “we are our stage of life.”
It must also be noted that some theorists, such as Alfred Adler, downplayed the assumption that every person went through the same stages in the same order. This matched with his focus on the uniqueness and creativity of each individual. More recently, feminist and postmodern theorists have questioned whether stages of personality, cognition, or moral development apply universally or only to the men or specific ethnic or cultural group on which the theory was based. When a certain developmental pattern is accepted as “normal,” then people who differ (remember, “no people are the same”) may think of themselves or be treated pejoratively as “abnormal.”
Despite these criticisms, the major emphasis of developmental psychology has been on understanding the characteristic changes in appearance, behavior, interests, and goals from one developmental period to another. Early research concentrated on schoolchildren. Later, interest spread to preschool, then infancy. Shortly after World War I, research studies of the adolescent years began. Between then and World War II, there were some studies of the early adult years, but they concentrated mainly on specific problems. Adulthood, middle age, and old age were ignored; it was as if no changes took place between adolescence and death. In the 1970s, books and journals began to appear that indicated stages in adult life that were as important as the stages in a child’s life.
With the exceptions of parent coaching and coaching with educators, coaches are more likely to use theories of development focused on adults.
Erik Erikson (1902-1994) was one theorist who emphasized developmental change throughout the human life span, not just in childhood or adolescence. In Erikson’s theory (Coles, 1970), eight stages of development unfold as we go through life. Each stage consists of a crisis that must be faced. According to Erikson, this crisis is not a catastrophe but a turning point of increased vulnerability and enhanced potential. The more successfully an individual resolves the crises, the healthier his or her development will be.
The stages include:
1. Trust versus mistrust, experienced in the first year of life.
2. Autonomy versus shame and doubt, occurring in late infancy and toddlerhood (1 to 3 years).
3. Initiative versus guilt, occurring during the preschool years.
4. Industry versus inferiority, which takes place from age 6 to age 11 and involves the shift from whimsical play to a desire for achievement and completion.
5. Identity versus identity confusion, when adolescents begin to seek their true identities and a sense of self (Erikson coined the phrase “identity crisis”).
6. Intimacy versus isolation, experienced during the early adulthood years.
7. Generativity versus stagnation, a crisis of middle adulthood.
8. Integrity versus despair, faced during late adulthood.
It can be enormously helpful for a coach to reframe a client’s midcareer questions as an opportunity to generate new goals or to confirm forgotten values.
Frederic Hudson has also contributed to the theoretical understanding of adult development (Hudson, 1999; Murphy & Hudson, 1995). Called “Dr. Midlife” by the LA Times, Hudson has brought a welcome theoretical depth to pioneering work in coaching.
Otto Laske based his Interdevelopmental (IDM) Institute coach training on what he calls a “Constructive Developmental Framework” (Laske, 2007). Laske claims that coaches must themselves pay attention to their own developmental level. Laske claims that “Interventions are developmentally counter-productive wherever the coach resides at a lower stage than does the client” (p. 239). Laske’s emphasis on values and his appreciation of constructivism and dialectic method are indications of developmental psychology’s contribution to the shift to a systemic worldview.

Linking Developmental Psychology to Coaching

• Developmental changes, crises, and urges can be important motivators, and coaches should keep developmental stages in mind when dealing with clients. A woman of 25 may well have different goals and a different opportunity for development from a woman of 50.
• It may be helpful for clients to reframe issues in their lives as opportunities to explore the next developmental phase.
• When facing the crisis of generativity versus stagnation, many organizational leaders begin to ask about their legacy, and legacy coaching can be of tremendous value at this point in their lives.
• However, just as we pointed out in the discussion of psychometrics, the essence of a coaching relationship is one in which two unique human beings collaborate in conversations that respect that uniqueness. No person—adult or child—is fully represented by a description of his or her developmental stage, however much people may benefit from exploring whether that stage applies to them and what it may mean specifically to them.
• An argument for coaches having their own coach and continuing to develop professionally is that we may inhibit our clients’ development by not continuing to grow beyond our own limits, what we call in this book “potentiating.”

EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Charles Darwin (1809-1882) was a British naturalist who shocked the 19th century with his book On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, first published in 1859. He proposed that through millions of years, all species of plants and animals had evolved from a few common ancestors. Natural selection theory rests on the assumption of inherited variations within a species. Certain variations aid those who have them in adapting to their environment, especially when the environment changes. The offspring of these individuals produce more offspring than others. Traits that hinder adaptation for the individual who carries them are eventually eliminated. Over time, these changes accumulate and the species evolves.
HISTORICAL INTERLUDE
Herbert Spencer (1820-1903) was an influential force for social psychological thought at the end of the 19th century. Spencer extended Darwin’s notions from the biological realm into the social. It was Spencer, not Darwin, who coined the phrase “survival of the fittest.” His espousal of Social Darwinism became quite influential in the thought of many early American psychologists, including William James.
The social Darwinist idea that behavior (not just physical traits) could be influenced by heredity was in direct opposition to Watson’s idea that environment, or conditioning, accounted entirely for personality and other psychological traits. Watson’s position that nurture trumped nature did not go unchallenged. William McDougall (1871-1938), a British psychologist who became professor of psychology at Harvard University, emphasized the role of instincts in human behavior and made it clear that he had little use for behaviorism or its resulting radical emphasis on nurture.
In 1924, the Psychology Club, a group of amateurs and professionals who were interested in psychological questions, took advantage of the enmity between the two theorists and arranged for a debate in Washington, DC, that was billed as “The Battle of Behaviorism.” McDougall and Watson each presented his argument, pulling out all the stops to convince the audience. The vote was taken, and McDougall won. But not only was his victory narrowly won, he ended up losing the war for popular and even academic influence. It was Watson’s behaviorism that fit with early-20th-century North American assumptions that science and technology could cure all social ills.
But the nature side of the dichotomy did not disappear. As part of the ideology that justified racism in the United States, “eugenics” was proposed as a way to maintain inherited physical and mental advantages. Nazism applied the theory systematically, first by eliminating political opponents, such as union leaders and socialists, and then by extending the policy to Jews and Gypsies. Along with the defeat of Nazi Germany in World War II, eugenics was discredited.
However, similar underlying assumptions and further discoveries in genetics fueled the development of sociobiology, a field that fell to perhaps its lowest point with the publication of The Bell Curve (Herrnstein & Murray, 1994). This book cited questionable statistics to revive the theory of racial inferiorities.
Konrad Lorenz (1903-1989) was an Austrian ornithologist and animal psychologist whose work on imprinting, modeling, and comparing animal and human psychology was influential in bringing biology and psychology into the same realm. He was awarded the 1973 Nobel Prize in Physiology and Medicine (sharing it with Nikolaas Tinbergen and Karl von Frisch) for discoveries in ethology.
Michael T. Ghiselin, an American philosopher and historian of biology, renewed the use of the term “evolutionary psychology” in a Science article published in 1973. Leda Cosmides from the Department of Psychology and John Tooby from the Department of Anthropology at the University of California Santa Barbara, began the Center for Evolutionary Psychology in the 1990s. In their online publication, Evolutionary Psychology: A Primer (1997) they quote Darwin’s Origin of Species: “In the distant future I see open fields for far more important researches. Psychology will be based on a new foundation, that of the necessary acquirement of each mental power and capacity by gradation” (in Cosmides & Tooby, 1997, p. 1). Cosmides and Tooby rely on this quote to support their claim that psychological traits, as well as physical traits, are governed by the scientific principles of evolution.
Evolutionary psychology understands human behavior as a result of the human brain’s evolution via natural selection. According to Cosmides and Tooby (1997), “Psychology is that branch of biology that studies (1) brains, (2) how brains process information, and (3) how the brain’s information-processing programs generate behavior.” They define the mind as “a set of information-processing machines” (p. 1). Their reduction of the mind to activities of the brain and their identification of the brain with a machine, albeit a computer, locates evolutionary psychologists firmly in the mechanistic paradigm.
Matt Ridley attempts to step beyond mechanism by resolving the nature/nurture debate. In Genome: The Autobiography of a Species in 23 Chapters (1999), Ridley celebrates the mapping of the human genome, comparing it to our species discovering its own recipe, with genes as the active ingredient. But Steven Quartz and Terry Sejnowski (2002) point out that experience, such as seeing different sights, turns on genes that build our visual pathways. A parent’s touch stimulates the development of genes that protect offspring against stress. “Genes, then, are the tools experience uses to change the brain’s response to new demands in new environments” (Quartz & Sejnowsky, 2002, p. 46). Nature and nurture cannot be disconnected.
What human beings inherit is the potential to produce a brain. Our brains are much more adaptable than was once believed, so adaptable that we cannot be said to “inherit” them. Rather, we inherit the capacity to participate, as Leslie Brothers (2001) puts it, to reformulate the details of the recipe as we are putting the socially derived ingredients together. The mind, unlike what would be possible in a purely Newtonian universe, is able to take a conscious and influential part in the mixing and making. In their book Mean Genes (2001), authors Terry Burnham and Jay Phelan argue that to understand human behavior, we need to stop looking at Freud and start looking at Darwin. Modern neuroscience suggests that we need to look beyond both Freud and Darwin to a new paradigm.
This is not easy to do, since the claims of evolutionary psychologists often seem so commonsense. One of these claims is that our brains have evolved little since we left the African savannah. Cosmides and Tooby (1997) state a major principle of evolutionary psychology: “Our modern skulls house a stone age mind” (p. 1). The claim is that many of our current everyday mental habits were created in response to environments that we no longer live in. Steven Rose (2005) lists several problems with this claim:
• The characterization of “Stone Age” life in the writing of evolutionary psychologists, while couched in scientific language, is often based more on Flintstone comic strip portrayals than on science.
• The idea that certain traits were fixed “back then” (whenever that might be) and could not possibly have changed is questionable because we do not know how fast it is possible for natural selection to operate.
• The hindsight conclusion that the fittest members of a species are the ones whose genes appear in subsequent generations is tautological—the very definition of “fitness” depends on whether genes “determining” that trait show up in a later gene pool.
• Because human minds and activity may affect inheritance, we cannot attribute fitness to genetics alone.
• The modular computer analogy ignores the importance of integration, which is increasingly the focus of neuroscience theory.
• Brains/minds do not just process information; they are concerned with meaning.
• Anthropologists among so-called primitive peoples from the 1970s report very different behavior as compared with the 1990s. Thus, how can we assume that the 1970s behavior represents ancient Stone Age traits?
• The one thing we know for sure is that the human brain is enormously adaptable. It is also highly social, with a vast potential for cooperation.
How can we who are not experts in these fields evaluate the claims and counter claims that surround evolutionary psychology? First of all, we can recognize that the new scientific paradigm that forms a theme throughout this book transcends either-or, nature-nurture, genes-environment dichotomies. That is, brains and physical bodies, minds, and environment have co-evolved. As brains and bodies have adapted to changing environments, minds have changed in response, and environments have changed as a result of the evolving behavior. In a linear sentence, there is no way to show the arrows of influence and adaptation going in all directions. Even the most complex graphic illustration, with bidirectional arrows connecting every element, is still just a snapshot of a moment in time that cannot capture the dynamism of an interacting system over time. It may be that the next challenge faced by our evolving thinking processes is how to conceptualize such complexity.

Linking Evolutionary Psychology to Coaching

Coaches have immediate concerns of how to help clients achieve their goals in the material world as it (temporarily) exists. Evolutionary psychology is popular because it seems to explain some of the difficulties human beings have with change.
For example, Burnham and Phelan (2000) ascribe modern tendencies to eat the wrong food to “powerful, instinctual hunger” that enabled Stone Age ancestors to survive by eating as much as they could any time that they could. We can leave aside possible contrary examples of plentitude illustrated by “potlatches” on the North American West Coast in more recent history. Certainly anyone who has tried to maintain a strict diet knows the feeling of “powerful, instinctual hunger.” If attributing this to our “Stone Age mind” helps us eat more healthful meals, then perhaps it is a harmless misapprehension.
Evolutionary psychology suggestions on diet can be applied to being served food on a plane that includes a brownie for dessert. Our Stone Age “instinctual hunger” for the brownie can be overcome by immediately opening a package of mustard and smearing it over the brownie so as not to be tempted.
As the section on change theory in this book has shown, rather than attributing our behavior to genetic inheritance from the Stone Age, modern neuroscience suggests other ways to explain difficulty with change, ways that are being directly tested and proved rather than surmised in hindsight.
The rediscovery of Darwinian principles has helped speed a resolution to the dualistic conflict that arises from the question “Are we determined by nature or by nurture?” In short, the answer is both and neither. As evidence of Alfred Adler’s forward thinking, he got it right nearly a century ago when he wrote, in awkward English,
Do not forget the most important fact that not heredity and not environment are determining factors—Both are giving only the frame and the influences which are answered by the individual in regard to his styled creative power. (Ansbacher & Ansbacher, 1956, p. xxiv)
The discovery of neuroplasticity means that neither nature, or heredity, nor environment, or nurture, determines who we are. Seeing the two as linked in a dialectic process is part of a shift to a systemic paradigm. Both nature and nurture set limits and contribute influences, but complex self-determining systems such as human beings and their societies are capable of demonstrating emergent properties. Evolutionary psychologists tend to see the mind as “caused” by an information-processing machinelike brain.
Attributing modern-day difficulties to having inherited a “Stone Age” brain and behavior may help clients avoid self-blame, but the scientific basis for this claim is questionable.

PSYCHOLOGY AS BEDROCK FOR COACHING

The attempt to establish a scientific study of human behavior serves as part of the bedrock for coaching. Although much of psychological research, especially in the early part of the 20th century, has been limited by the requirement for objectivity that comes from the mechanistic worldview, many principles continue to be valid:
• Behaviorist principles guide many coaching programs and contribute to the attempt to define competencies in behavioral terms.
• Psychometric assessments are a major focus of the change strategies for many coaches.
• Developmental psychology has provided concepts for exploring client issues over their life span.
• The claims of evolutionary psychology can be seen as warnings to stick close to evidence rather than taking speculative leaps.
In 1956, a new cognitive approach replaced the association of stimulus and response that guided Skinner and his predecessors. Learning theory grew beyond its assumptions of passive receivers of knowledge. These new approaches assumed an active mind and provided a pillar to support the systemic paradigm, coaching, and neuroscience.
..................Content has been hidden....................

You can't read the all page of ebook, please click here login for view all page.
Reset
18.117.71.189