Ascan F. Koerner

18 Family communication

Abstract: This chapter overviews social-scientific family communication research. Particular emphasis is given to empirical research designed to test theoretically-derived hypotheses and to research that has originated within the communication discipline. After briefly discussing the centrality of families to human relationships and the centrality of communication to family relationships, two general theories of family communication are reviewed, followed by a review of theories more narrowly focused on specific types of family communication. Empirical findings that buttress knowledge claims concerning family communication are reported. The main focus, however, is on the underlying theories or models motivating the research. The chapter concludes with a summary evaluation highlighting the strengths and weaknesses of the research as well as predictions about future developments in the family communication research area.

 

Key Words: affect, corporal punishment, family communication patterns, family conflict, family functioning, family secretes, family types, family violence, parenting, privacy

1 The scientific investigation of family communication

The investigation of families has a long tradition in Western social sciences, mainly because the family is considered a pivotal social group that is foundational to many social institutions and to societies at large. Equally important, the family is where much of the socialization of subsequent generations takes place (cf. Harris 1998). Thus, understanding families, family dynamics, and family communication is important in understanding larger social processes. Simultaneously, because the family as an institution is closely tied to many other social institutions, understanding these and how they are related to families leads to insights into family functioning and psychosocial outcomes for family members. It is no surprise, then, that a substantial part of the social scientific research on families originated with disciplines that take a sociological approach to inquiry, such as sociology, economics, and family social sciences.

Other research on families that has historically made great contribution to our understanding of families, family dynamics, and family communication originated with more psychologically-oriented disciplines, such as child development and social psychology. Researchers in these disciplines have focused on the roles families play in the development of children and young adults, as well as in the fact that family relationships, for most persons, are the most enduring, most intimate, and thus most significant interpersonal relationships they have. Consequently, family relationships greatly affect their mental and physiological functioning and wellbeing. At the same time, reciprocal relationships exist here as well. Families are constituted by individual members whose emotional and cognitive functioning are affected by family processes and dynamics, but whose individual emotions and cognitions also have the ability to affect other family members as well as family level processes and dynamics.

As a consequence of the importance of family relationships and their dynamics for the functioning and well being of individuals, institutions, and societies, family relationships, their functioning, and their interconnectedness with other institutions are well researched. Unfortunately, this does not necessarily mean that family communication per se is as well researched. This apparent contradiction can be explained by that fact that although family communication dynamics are clearly central in linking family members and processes to either individual emotion and cognition on the one hand, and to institutions, social processes, and societies on the other hand, most researchers in either the sociological or psychological traditions tend to treat communication as a “primitive” term, that is, a term not in need of theoretical elaboration and conceptualization. In these traditions, communication more often than not is treated as obvious and self-evident (“what you see is what you get“) (Caughlin 2010; Caughlin and Scott 2010; Caughlin and Vangelisti 2006; Koerner and Fitzpatrick 2013). Consequently, while it is assumed that communication links individuals, families, and social institutions with one another, how communication does this is not necessarily clear from much of this research.

Thus, it has fallen to communication scholars to define and to theorize about family communication in more theoretical and social scientific ways. Within the discipline of communication, however, the use of social scientific theories and methods to study family communication is a relatively recent development. Although research focusing specifically on family communication and family relationships has been published early on in communication journals and handbooks and thus been part of the field from its beginning, the National Communication Association has had a Family Communication division only since 1995 and the International Communication Association still lacks a family communication division. Likewise, the first academic journal exclusively devoted to family communication (the Journal of Family Communication) was not published until 2001 and the first handbook of family communication was not published until 2004 (Vangelisti 2004).

These slow beginnings notwithstanding, interest in and the amount of published social scientific research of family communication has grown tremendously over the last two decades, even though the proportion of empirical research of all research in family communication published in major communication and interdisciplinary journals has declined somewhat from about 80% in 1990–1994 to about 68% in 2005–2009 (Stamp and Shue 2013). This interest in scientific research of families has brought with it the development of several serious theories and models of family communication, as well as a significant increase in more rigorous social scientific studies of family communication. While these models and theories do not yet rest on the depth and breadth of empirical knowledge comparable to that of more established areas of interpersonal communication research, progress has been made and its future looks bright. In fact, many of the theories developed and investigated in the area of family communication are quite ambitious in their scope and theoretical depth.

Owing to the large amount of social scientifically motivated research on family communication, it is impossible to provide a comprehensive overview of it in a single chapter. Readers interested in such reviews are referred to edited volumes on family communication by Braithwaite and Baxter (2006) and Vangelisti (2013). Rather, this review focuses on the most scientifically rigorous research on family communication; that is, research that is not only empirical, but also aims to test well developed and clearly articulated theoretical models. Work that originated with the field of communication or that has had significantly shaped the research in the discipline will receive particular emphasis. Although the marital relationship constitutes an important dyadic relationship within families and married and cohabitating couples constitute families according to most definitions of family (Koerner and Fitzptrick 2013), they are not considered in this chapter as there is a chapter devoted specifically to marriages in this handbook (see Chapter 19, Segrin and Flora). The chapter will conclude with by considering likely future developments in the family communication domain.

2 General family communication theories

Arguably, research about a phenomenon becomes scientific when objective, quantifiable observations are used to test specific predictions derived from clearly delineated theoretical models. Currently, few family communication theories meet this – admittedly high – standard, and even fewer theories that make claims to be about family communication in general rather than being about specific types of family communication fit this description. General theories of family communication describe processes that apply to most, if not all, interactions within families. Thus, they focus on those basic aspects of communication that underlie any interaction, such as sense-making or goal achievement. These processes are fundamental to communication in general, rather than applying to only specific types of interactions, such as conflict or support, for example. They are specific to families because they are based on fundamental processes that are unique, or uniquely relevant, to family relationships.

2.1 Olson’s circumplex model

One early family communication theory that emerged from an allied discipline but that made theorizing about the role of communication in family process central and therefore has received considerable attention is Olson’s circumplex model of family functioning (1981, 1993; Olson, Sprenkle and Russell 1979; Olson, Russell and Sprenkle 1983). Olsen argued that family functioning is determined by two fundamental dimensions of families: cohesion and adaptability. According to the theory, moderate levels on both dimensions are associated with superior functioning, whereas extremes on either the dimensions are associated with less than optimal functioning. For cohesion, this means that separated or engaged families function better than disengaged or enmeshed families, and for adaptation that flexible or structured families function better than rigid or chaotic families. Families that are moderate on both dimensions function best, followed by families that are moderate on one but extreme on the other dimension, and families that are extreme on both dimensions are the least functional.

Family communication is identified by Olson (1981, 1993) as a third, facilitating dimension of families. Family’s communication ultimately determines where the family falls along the cohesion and adaptability dimensions. Communication also allows families to change their location along the dimensions, which is particularly important for the application of the circumplex model to family therapy. Once a counselor has established where along the two dimensions a dysfunctional family falls, the counselor can suggest specific communication behaviors that may move a family toward more moderate levels on the dimensions, thereby increasing family functioning.

Despite the central, even deterministic role of communication in Olsons’s model, the mechanisms by which communication facilitates the dimensions of cohesion and adaptability were left basically undefined. Although Olson identified specific communication skills (or behaviors) as facilitating moderate levels of cohesion and adaptability, include speaking skills such as speaking for self and avoiding speaking for others, listening skills such as active listening and empathy, as well as general communication skills such as self disclosure, clarity, continuity and tracking, and showing respect and regard for other, how and why these communication behaviors would lead to moderate levels of adaptability and cohesion was never fully explicated. Even more problematic, Olson’s own research produced linear rather than the predicted curvilinear relationships between the communication behaviors and cohesion and adaptability, respectively. Partially as a consequence, family communication researchers’ interest in Olson’s circumplex model has weaned considerably in recent years.

2.2 Family communication patterns theory

A general theory of family communication that originated from within the communication discipline is Family Communication Patterns Theory (FCPT) (Fitzpatrick and Ritchie 1994; Koerner 2007; Koerner and Fitzpatrick 2002a, 2002bc, 2004, 2006a; Ritchie and Fitzpatrick 1990). Grounded in cognitive theory, FCPT postulates that creating a shared social reality is a process fundamental to family functioning that defines family relationships and determines how families communicate. Shared social reality in families is created through two communication behaviors: conversation orientation and conformity orientation, which together determine families’ communication patterns. Conversation orientation refers to open and frequent communication between parents and children with the purpose of co-discovering and co-determining the meaning of objects that constitute social reality. It is associated with warm and supportive communication characterized by mutual respect and concern for one another. Conformity orientation, in contrast, refers to more restricted communication between parents and children in which those of authority, typically the parents, define the family’s social reality. Conformity orientation is associated with more authoritarian parenting and less concern for the children’s thoughts and feelings.

These two orthogonal orientations toward creating shared social reality define a conceptual space for family communication describing four family types. Consensual families are high on both conversation orientation and conformity orientation. Their communication is characterized by a tension between an interest in open communication and in exploring new ideas, on the one hand, and pressure to agree and to preserve the existing hierarchy of the family, on the other hand. Parents attempt to solve this tension by listening to and by explaining their values and beliefs to their children and by trying to persuade them to adopt their views. Children in these families are usually satisfied and well adapted.

Pluralistic families have high conversation orientation and low conformity orientation. Their communication is characterized by open, unconstrained discussions that involve all family members and a wide range of topics. Parents in these families exert little control over their children and welcome their children’s different beliefs and values, although they also explain their own values and beliefs to them. Children of these families learn to be independent and autonomous and to communicate persuasively and they are generally satisfied with their family relationships.

Protective families have high conformity and low conversation orientation. Their communication is characterized by obedience to parental authority and by little concern for conceptual matters. Parents in these families exert strong control over their children and rarely explain their reasoning to them. They express their beliefs and values and expect their children to adopt them as well. Children in protective families learn that there is little value in family conversations and to distrust their own decision making ability.

Laissez-Faire families are low on both conversation and conformity orientation. Their communication is characterized by few, uninvolving interactions. Laissez-faire families are emotionally distant and family members have little interest in the thoughts and feeling of other family members. Children of these families learn not to value family conversations and to become independent decision makers. Because they do not receive parental support or encouragement, they distrust their own decision making ability and are particularly susceptible to peer influence.

These family communication patterns that stem from how families create shared social reality have been associated with a number of family processes, such as confirmation and affection (Schrodt, Ledbetter, and Ohrt 2007), conflict resolution (Koerner and Fitzpatrick 1997), family rituals (Baxter and Clark 1996) and understanding (Sillars, Koerner, and Fitzpatrick 2005), as well as with child-related outcomes, such as communication apprehension (Elwood and Shrader 1998), conflict with romantic partners (Koerner and Fitzpatrick 2002c), children’s mental and physical health (Schrodt and Ledbetter 2007), and children’s resiliency (Fitzpatrick and Koerner 2005).

3 Theories about specific types of family communication

Only a few family communication theories aim to be general theories that apply to virtually all communication contexts. Rather, the majority of family communication theories address more circumscribed types of family communication. The advantages of these more focused theories is that they often allow for more precise predictions and a more fine-grained understanding of the communication processes involved in specific types of interactions.

3.1 Parenting and child socialization

Probably the most essential characteristic of family is the raising of children (Koerner and Fitzpatrick 2013). Thus, it should come as no surprise that much of the research and theorizing about family communication focuses on parenting and the socialization of children. Generally, parenting and socialization have been identified as having two primary functions. The first is to enable children to become socially competent and to perform successfully in their respective social roles. To fulfill this function, families must be oriented externally toward social relationships and be concerned with social norms and rules and adherence to them. This process is fundamentally about compliance with social rules and norms. The other function is for children to achieve a certain degree of psychological well-being that is indicated by them exhibiting satisfaction with their lives, their relationships, and their selves and experience a general sense of happiness. To fulfill this second function, families orient inwards toward acceptance and love of self and are concerned with children’s self-esteem. This process is fundamentally about love and acceptance (Koerner and Fitzpatrick 2013).

This view is echoed by Steinberg (2001), who reviewed two decades of parenting research and concluded that the two most consistent findings of this research have been that warm and firm parenting that is responsive to children’s needs for autonomy is associated with positive child adjustment (Steinberg 1990; Steinberg, Elmen, and Mounts 1989). This generalization is also supported by four decades of parenting research based on Baumrind’s parenting styles (1967, 1971, 1991), which consistently demonstrated that authoritative parenting, which combines warmth with discipline, is superior in terms of child outcomes to permissive parenting (warmth without discipline ), authoritarian parenting (discipline without warmth), and neglecting parenting (neither warmth nor discipline). Other researchers have used different labels to describe these two core concepts of parenting, the results, however, are the same.

Another group of researchers investigated the role parenting strategies play in the social and cognitive development of children (Burleson, Delia and Applegate 1995). According to these scholars, parents use two different communication strategies to regulate children’s behaviors: A person-centered approach or a position-centered approach. In the person-centered approach, parents employ regulating and comforting messages to their children that emphasize how other people are affected by the child’s behavior and that focuses on the needs, values, feelings, and psychology of others. In contrast, the position-centered approach employs regulating and comforting messages that emphasize rules and norms that apply regardless of whether or how others are affected by the behavior. The person-centered approach leads children to develop communication skills that enhance their ability to be empathetic and to take other person’s perspective into consideration, which leads children to develop complex and sophisticated mental representations of themselves, others, and their interpersonal relationships (Burleson et al. 1995; Young 2009). In contrast, the position-centered approach leads children to develop communication skills that enhance their ability to identify pertinent social rules and norms, without considering the other persons’ perspectives. Such communication leads children to develop less complex and less sophisticated and more rigid mental representations of themselves, others, and interpersonal relationships (Burleson et al. 1995; Young 2009).

3.2 Family secrets

Research devoted to exploring even more specific types of family communication is exemplified by Vangelisti’s work on family secrets (1994, 1997; Vangelisti, Caughlin, and Timmerman 2001). According to these researchers, family secrets can be differentiated by form, topic, and function (Vangelisti 1994). Forms vary from whole family secrets (all family members know but outsiders do not) to intra-family secret (some family members know, but others and outsiders do not) to individual secrets (only one family member knows, but other family members and outsiders do not). Topics vary from the conventional (e.g., children’s grades, illness of a parent) to rule violations (e.g., alcohol use, out of wedlock pregnancy of a family member) to taboos (e.g., marital difficulties, physical and sexual abuse). Functions of secrets identified by Vangelisti (1994) include bonding, evaluation, maintenance, privacy, defense and communication.

Vangelisti’s (1994) found correlations between secret form, topic, and function and family satisfaction. For example, taboo secrets were most likely to be whole family secrets and least likely to be individual secrets, whereas rule violations were most likely to be individual secrets and least likely whole family secrets. There was no association between form and topic for conventional secrets. As far as the association between form and function was concerned, Vangelisti found that whole family secrets mainly served the evaluative, defense, and privacy functions; intra-family secrets mainly served the maintenance function; while individual secrets mainly served the evaluative and privacy functions.

Regarding the revelation of family secrets to outsiders, Vangelisti (1997; Vangelisti, Caughlin and Timmermann 2001) found that a member’s satisfaction with their family relationships was negatively associated with revealing secrets to outsiders, whereas closeness and similarity with the outsider was positively associated with revealing family secretes. These findings suggest that family secrets are used to manage interpersonal relationships both within families and with outsiders. Revealing secrets reduces the importance and intimacy of family relationships and family members can use such revelations to assert greater autonomy and independence from the families. At the same time, revealing family secrets also increases the intimacy and interdependence of the family member’s relationship with the outsider and family members can use revealing secretes to increase the closeness of these relationships.

3.3 Privacy management theory

Closely related to the work on family secrets is communication privacy management theory (CPMT) (Petronio 2002, 2010; Petronio and Caughlin 2006). Although it was not developed exclusively for the application to family communication, it is this context in which it has found it greatest use (Caughlin, Petronio, and Middleton 2013). Like Vangelisiti (1994, 1997), CPMT proposes that private information is used primarily to manage interpersonal relationships. According to Petronio (2002, 2010), private information, that is, information about oneself that is potentially harmful or embarrassing, “belongs” to the individual but can be “shared” with others who then obtain partial ownership of the information and share in the responsibility to safeguard it and to use it appropriately. This sharing of private information defines relationships boundaries of varying degrees of permeability and serves to solidify or weakening relationship bonds and keeps others within or outside the relationship (Petronio, 2010). In addition to affecting relationship boundaries, sharing private information also affects the quality of family relationships. Often, sharing enhances family relationships as it represents an expression of closeness, intimacy, and trust (Petronio 2002), but it can also decrease family satisfaction, especially if the information has the potential to negatively affect their relationships with other family members (Afifi, et al. 2007).

This perspective on private information and its strategic use to manage interpersonal and particularly family relationships represents a significant theoretical advancement over early theories of interpersonal communication and relationships, which regarded private information as essentially synonymous with intimacy and conceptualized relationships as being primarily about achieving greater intimacy (e.g., Altman and Taylor 1973)

3.4 Affective communication in families

Another research area of more focused scope involves investigations of the expression of affect in families. A particularly good example of scientific research is Affective Exchange Theory (AET) (Floyd 2001; Floyd and Morman 2003). Unlike many other family communication theories that explain phenomena but take the phenomena themselves as given, Floyd provided a theoretical explanation for why affect is exchanged in family relationships by explicitly grounding AET in evolutionary theory. AET argues that humans’ ability to experience and express affection was evolutionary selected for because it created significant benefits in terms of survival and reproduction. Thus, AET is tied to a larger, extremely powerful explanatory framework rather than standing on its own.

Tying AET to evolutionary theory not only places the theory into a larger, powerful explanatory framework that is relevant to, and used by, a number of social scientists, it also creates an environment that encourages the formulation and testing of very specific hypotheses. Furthermore, unlike hypotheses that originate from theoretical frameworks that were developed specifically for the phenomena they explain and therefore hew close to the empirical observations made about these phenomena, hypotheses based on more distal theoretical explanations have the chance to be truly counterintuitive. As such, data supporting them make a much more powerful case for their theoretical models than data supporting hypotheses that are not counterintuitive. For example, if the evolutionary reason for parental affection is to enable one’s offspring to survive and propagate, it follows that fathers should be more affectionate with biological as opposed to step-children (Floyd and Morman 2001). Likewise, fathers should also be more affectionate with heterosexual sons than with bisexual or homosexual sons, and this association should be mediated by their fathers’ perceptions of their sexual orientations (Floyd, 2001; Floyd, Sargent and Di Corcia 2004).

Floyd’s work on AFT does not only support an evolutionary framework, however. Through its investigating father-son relationships, Floyd and his associates described an often neglected family relationship in great detail and found, for example, that affection in father-son relationships is expressed more through supportive behavior than through direct verbal or non-verbal expression of affection (Floyd, Sargent and Di Corcia 2004). Also, Morman and Floyd (2002, 2006) were able to demonstrate the impact that changing cultural norms have on this family dyad by comparing current fathers’ reports on their relationships with their fathers to their and their sons’ reports on their current father-son relationships. These researchers found that both fathers and sons in contemporary relationships report much more affectionate communication than current fathers had with their own fathers (Floyd and Morman 2005). Thus, while evolved psychological processes do play an important role in father-son relationships, so too do cultural forces.

3.5 Family conflict

Unlike most interpersonal relationships that are voluntary and therefore rarely persist if they are very conflicted, family relationships are involuntary and often characterized by high incidents of conflict (Roloff and Miller 2006; Shantz and Hobart 1989; see Chapter 8, Canary and Canary), with conflict behavior significantly more frequent in family relationships than in peer or work relationships (Sillars, Canary and Tafoya 2004). One reason family relationships are conflicted is that a large proportion of parent-child communication consists of parents’ attempts to regulate their children’s behavior and children often resisting (Laursen 1993; Laursen and Collins 2004; Laursen, Coy and Collins 1998) and resenting these attempts (Collins and Luebker 1994, Smetana, Yau and Hanson 1991). At the same time, family relationships are highly interdependent with family members depending on each other in achieving important individual and interpersonal goals, increasing the potential for families to experience frustration and related negative affect that often finds its expression in conflict of other forms of hostility (Clark, Fitness and Brisette 2001; Fitness 2013).

3.5.1 Functions of family conflict

Thus, parent-child conflict in particular has to be considered a normal, if not necessarily desirable aspect of family communication. It necessarily results when parents’ legitimate interest in regulating their children’s behaviors collides with their children’s legitimate interest in autonomy and self regulation. Young children often are less invested in their autonomy and regard their parents’ regulation as legitimate and therefore are more accepting of it. Adolescents, on the other hand, are more invested in their autonomy and therefore more likely not only to resist their parents, but also experience more negative affect than younger children, which makes parent-adolescent conflict often more volatile (Laursen, Coy and Collins 1998). Thus, not only is parent-child conflict inevitable, it serves important developmental functions, especially for adolescents. Noller (1995) identified five such functions, all of which are mainly achieved through conflict communication: renegotiate roles, rules, and relationships; explore identity; enhance self-esteem; model and teach problem solving; and enable decision making.

Family conflict not only helps children to develop into adulthood, it also socializes children to adopt particular conflict styles in their extra-familial interpersonal relationships (Koerner and Fitzpatrick 1997, 2002c; Noller 1995; Rinaldi and Howe 2003). Children will employ similar communication styles not only in interactions within their families, but also in relationships outside the family (Monte-mayor and Hanson 1985) and in subsequent interpersonal relationships they have as adults as well (Koerner and Fitzpatrick 2002c; Palazzolo, Roberto and Babin 2010).

There is considerable evidence suggesting that children adopt the conflict styles of their parents. Montemayor and Hanson (1985) found that adolescents use similar conflict styles in relationships with their parents and in relationships with their siblings. Similarly, in a study comparing dyadic conflict styles of family members to that of adolescent children with their romantic partners, Reese-Weber and Bertle-Haring (1998) found that the conflict styles of parents and children were significantly correlated. Results of this study also showed that inter-parental conflict styles were indirectly related to children’s conflict style with siblings and their romantic partners. These associations were mediated by parent-adolescent conflict styles. Finally, research by Palazzolo, Roberto and Babin (2010) showed strong correlations between reported parental verbal aggression and adult children’s reports of intimate partner violence perpetration and victimization, respectively. Together, these findings suggest that parents use similar conflict styles with one another and with their children, who in turn use similar conflict styles in their interpersonal relationships within and outside the family.

Similarities between parents and children are not limited to conflict behavior. Collecting data on conflict behavior and perceptions within and outside families, Rinaldi and Howe (2003) found that behaviors and perceptions are highly correlated. Rather than just learning conflict communication styles from one another on the behavioral level, family members also share their perceptions of family conflict. Thus, it is not only specific conflict behaviors that children are socialized to in families, but rather their entire perception of how families communicate with one another and the role that conflict plays in family life. Research by Koerner and associates (Koerner and Fitzpatrick 1997, 2002c; Koerner and Maki 2004) also supports the idea that conflict behaviors are not learned in isolation but are part of more general family communication socialization patterns.

3.5.2 Consequences of family conflict

Although family conflict undoubtedly affects the well-being of all family members, research has focused almost exclusively on its effects on children; knowledge about its effects on parents is extremely limited (Koerner 2013). For children the evidence suggests that destructive, hostile, or even violent family conflict is associated with negative outcomes for adolescents (Smetana, Yau and Hanson 1991), and even more so for younger children (Rhoades 2008). Young children exposed to intense, destructive parental conflict experience fear, sadness, and anger (Cummings, Iannotti and Zahn-Waxler 1985) and feel less safe and secure in the home and outside the family (Gordis, Margolin and John 2001). Young children of families that frequently engage in destructive conflict also experience problems in their peer and sibling relationships (Jenkins 2000), such as greater aggression and anger in relationships with teachers and peers and siblings (Campione-Barr and Smetana 2010; Noller 1995).

In contrast, to the extent to which families engage in constructive conflict that is rational and focused on problem solving, family conflict is associated with positive outcomes for children, and, in particular, adolescents. Children of parents who exhibit positive problem solving behaviors during family conflict report more favorable psychological outcomes (Cummings et al. 1985), have better peer relationships (Jenkins 2000), and generally are better adjusted (Tucker, McHale and Crouter 2003). Because children experience safety and security in their relationships with parents, they are psychologically better adapted, have greater self-esteem and perceive the world to be a more friendly and less threatening place. In regard to social outcomes, the conflict styles they learned from their parents help them to manage their relationships with their peers more successfully.

3.6 Family violence

Family relationships are not only the most conflicted, but also the most physically violent interpersonal relationships most persons have (Straus 1990, 1994; Straus and Gelles 1990). Despite significant changes in U.S. social norms over the last 50 years that have made acts of severe violence (including kicking, punching, and hitting with objects such as belts or paddles) unacceptable or even illegal in spousal, parent-child, and sibling relationships, a 2008 nationally representative sample showed 18% of children and adolescents being exposed to physical violence in their homes, 10% saw a parent hit by a partner, and 5% saw their parent physically assaulted by their partner (Hamby et al. 2011). In a survey on sibling relationships, 36% of children and adolescents reported physically violence in conflict with a sibling (Finkelhor, Turner and Hamby 2005).

3.6.1 Conflict and violence

Not all family conflict involves violence, but conflict and violence are related in that family violence is usually, but not always, preceded by (nonviolent) conflict. In families that experience violence, the role of conflict in family violence is often that of a precursor. In such families physical violence is the continuation of interpersonal conflict by different means. Not all family violence, however, is necessarily preceded by conflict. Some forms of violence, such as consistent psychological, physical, or sexual abuse are manifestations of family processes unrelated to conflict.

Family conflict has been conceptualizaed as existing along a continuum of intensity, from low intensity problem solving at the one end of the continuum to high intensity physical fighting at the other end of the continuum (e.g., Straus 1990, 1994). In this conceptualization, violent family conflict can be explained as a function of the same factors that make nonviolent family conflict more intense. Factors that have been associated with increased intensity of conflict have been a greater involvement, that is, personal or relational significance of the conflict issues, and frustration resulting from the process of conflict communication itself (Donohue and Kolt 1992). For example, conflict intensity might increase because the conflict involves a family member’s needs rather than interests, where needs are defined as tied to personal and identity goals, whereas interests are defined as tied to instrumental goals. Alternatively, intensity can increase because one or more parties in the conflict become frustrated with the process of conflict communication (Gottman 1991, 1993) or the process itself threatens important needs, such as positive and negative face (Brown and Levinson 1987).

3.6.2 Consequences of family violence

Research aimed at understanding the outcomes of family violence has either investigated the outcomes of spousal abuse for battered spouses (e.g., Johnson and Ferraro 2000) or the outcomes for children (e.g., Gershoff 2002). Results showed that children who experience family violence are more likely to enact violence in their peer relationships and their subsequent adult relationships (Carr and VanDeusen 2002; Salzinger et al. 2002; Simons, Lin and Gordon 1998; Straus and Yodanis 1996; Swinford et al. 2000; Whitfield et al. 2003; Yexley, Borowsky and Ireland 2002). For example, Salzinger et al. (2002) described the effects of observed partner violence and physical abuse on children and found that abused children exhibited significantly higher levels of meanness and fighting and were six times more likely to be rated by peers and teachers as antisocial. Likewise, Carr and VanDeusen (2002) examined interparental violence and parent-child aggression as predictors of intimate partner violence of young men in a college sample. Results indicated that witnessing inter-parental violence and experiencing childhood violence predicted physical violence in subsequent dating relationships.

There are several theoretical explanations linking family violence to the negative psychological and social outcomes for children. Most researchers investigating the effects of family violence employ a model of intergenerational transmission of violence. This model proposes that children who experience or witness violence in their families become socialized to see interpersonal violence as a normal and expected part of interpersonal relationships and to act more violently in their own interpersonal relationships (Gil-González et al. 2008; Simons et al. 1998; Straus and Yodanis 1996; Swinford et al. 2000). Experiencing family violence not only leads children to enact more violence in their own interpersonal relationships, but also to become more tolerant of interpersonal violence enacted by their partners. Thus, children exposed to physical violence within the family are at an increased risk for both increased perpetration of interpersonal violence as well as for increased victimization (Whitfield et al. 2003)

3.7 Corporal punishment

One form of family violence that is unique for both its ubiquitousness (Holden, Miller and Harris 1999) and the contentiousness with which research regarding it is received by the public is corporal punishment. According to Strauss (1990), corporal punishment usually is defined as “the use of physical force with the intention of causing a child to experience pain, but not injury, for the purpose of correction or control of the child’s behavior” (p. 4), differentiating it from violence, which is defined as “an act carried out with the intention, or perceived intention, of causing physical pain or injury to another person” (p. 7). Because this distinction depends on the (perceived) intention of the actor, which might not necessarily be obvious or unambiguous, it is difficult for both participants and observers to determine whether an act was corporal punishment or violence.

Potentially due to the difficulty to distinguish between corporal punishment and interpersonal violence, the outcomes for children experiences are remarkably similar (but see Baumrind, Larzelere and Cowan 2002). In an extensive review of research on corporal punishment, Gershoff (2002) found that although corporal punishment often results in short-term compliance, the long-term outcomes of corporal punishment for children are similar to those of other forms of family violence. Such outcomes included increased aggression, delinquency, and antisocial behaviors, decreased moral internalization, lower quality of parent-child relationships, and mental health problems for children (Gershoff 2002). Outcomes that follow children into adulthood included increased risks for adult criminal and antisocial behavior and for abusing one’s own children (Gershoff 2002; Straus 1994). Other research has linked corporal punishment to increased externalizing behaviors (McKenzie et al. 2011) and delayed cognitive development (Straus and Paschall 2009).

One explanation for the finding that violent family conflict and corporal punishment have similarly negative outcomes for children could be that regardless of parents’ intentions, the emotional and physical experience for children is extremely similar. Although parents might able to distinguish between violence and corporal punishment in terms of their own intention, their children might not be able to make this distinction because they necessarily rely on their own inferences and attributions when assigning intent to their parents’ behaviors (Sillars and Canary 2013).

Because most corporal punishment is administered by frustrated and angry parents, it represents a form of parental communication that is both emotionally upsetting and coercive. An alternative explanation for why the negative effects of corporal punishment are indistinguishable from those of parental violence is based on functional correlates of corporal punishment. The use of corporal punishment tends to preclude the opportunity for parents to utilize other, more socially useful methods of behavior control, such as reasoning and reflection and therefore results in children’s truncated development of conflict resolution skills (Straus and Yodanis 1996). The more parents rely on corporal punishment, the fewer opportunities children have to witness and participate in non-violent modes of influencing another person’s behavior, leaving them to rely on physical violence rather than more prosocial strategies. Like other types of family violence, corporal punishment teaches children problem resolution and conflict skills that they cannot successfully use in their relationships outside the family and that therefore impede their social competence and functioning.

4 Conclusion and future directions

4.1 Summary

This brief review of the social scientific investigation of family communication has shown that as a scholarly endeavor, family communication research has made some important contributions to our understanding of family relationships and processes. At the same time, it is still a rather immature science and the majority of empirical work in family communication is primarily concerned with establishing knowledge about associations between variables, especially in regard to child development and adjustment. Mostly, however, this work is not scientific in the sense that it tests hypotheses derived from sophisticated or even clearly articulated theoretical models. In fact, what many researchers call theories or models frequently amount to little more than the predicted associations themselves, and disappointingly often, the only justification for the prediction is that it has been observed before. If there are underlying mechanisms that are causally responsible for the predicted associations, they often are implicit rather than illuminated by a convincing theoretical explanation. It is doubtful that these scholars are unaware of what theory is or should be. Quite the opposite, they understand the value of theory in science and recognize that to be taken seriously in the field, research has to be theory driven, even if the collective empirical knowledge of family communication might be insufficient to develop full-fledged scientific theories about it. Thus, the relative lack of strong family communication theories has more to do with the youth of the discipline

Perhaps this review has shown that, despite misgivings about the relative scarcity of scientific research in family communication, there are examples of truly exceptional theoretical and empirical work that is being done. While this chapter has focused on the theoretical and empirical aspects of these research programs, they are often made possible by the development of tremendously sophisticated methods in recent years, particularly in regard to statistical analysis. Hypotheses testing and other types of empirical research in family communication can be challenging because data are often “messy” and there are various types of dependencies of observations that violate basic assumptions underlying more traditional statistical methods. Newer methods, such as hierarchically nested models, not only control for such dependencies, but can also identify individual level, dyadic, and family level effects, allowing for much greater precision in describing and testing associations between variables and at what level of analysis certain processes operate (see Chapter 5, Caughlin and Basinger; and Chapter 6, Liu).

Finally, this review has also shown the tremendous promise scientific research of family communication holds. All of the theories reviewed here aim to reach a level of complexity that is appropriate to the complexity of the phenomenon under investigation. At the beginning of the chapter, I noted that many allied disciplines treat communication as a primitive term. As communication scholars we know that communication is anything but. Messages and nonverbal behavior are imbued with meaning that depends not only the behaviors and symbols used, but individual cognitive and affective process, the physical space, its place in an ongoing interaction and relationship, the sociocultural context, and a host of other factors whose impact on meaning is negotiated in the very interactions they give meaning to.

The theories reviewed in this chapter recognize this complexity and attempt to account for it. For example, in Privacy Management Theory (Petronio 2002, 2010), relationship boundary permeability not only predicts with whom private information is shared, but with whom private information is shared also predicts the boundaries around family relationships, specifying a recursive association or an association in which causality can change direction. Clearly, one-directional causal associations are conceptually cleaner and easier to theorize about and to test empirically, but the actual phenomenon might not be reducible to linear causal explanation. While this makes privacy management theory more difficult to test, it also makes it a more valid and ultimately more powerful theory.

Similarly, family communication is not self-contained; rather, it is nested within a series of hierarchically ordered systems that are interdependent on one another. Theorizing about communication requires recognition of these systems and the effects they have on communication, even if that requires theorizing about processes that are seemingly far removed from the interaction or the family system. One example of such theorizing is Affective Exchange Theory (AET) (Floyd 2001; Floyd and Morman 2003), which recognizes that communicative behavior is impacted by cognitive processes that have evolved due to evolutionary pressures and that, however unconsciously, affect how we think and feel about our family members and, as a consequence, how we communicate with them. These evolutionary pressures may have taken place tens of thousands of years ago in families that were organized quite unlike ours, yet they still impact how modern families communicate today. In the end, the best scientific theories recognize and address the tremendous complexity of family communication and resists simple and simplistic explanations.

4.2 Future directions

While it is difficult to predict the future, at least in the short term two developments will significantly affect the scientific investigation of family communication. The first is that the theories motivating family communication research will become more complex to account for the complexity of the behaviors they seek to explain. In this process, there is a good chance that narrow theories will broaden and begin to make theoretical connections to other narrow theories that explain other, ostensibly distinct phenomena. For example, there is no good theoretical reason for why family conflict should be treated as distinct from corporal punishment, and why privacy management should be considered separate from parenting. Clearly, these phenomena are related and knowledge and theory about one phenomenon should be useful in understanding the other. Ultimately, it might even be possible that these narrow focused theories converge into broader, more comprehensive models that are based on “grand theories” of relationships, such as evolutionary theory.

The second development that will affect future research is greater awareness about nontraditional families and the greater social acceptance and visibility these families enjoy. The increased attention family communication researchers pay to non-traditional and complex families, such as single-parent, blended, step, and multi-generational families and families created through artificial reproductive technologies or adoption open research to investigate a whole new set of variables that deserve attention. Examples of such variables include cultural norms and expectations about marriages and families, perceptions of gender and sexual orientation, the role of self and family identities, relationships between biologically unrelated siblings and parents and children, to name just a few. Up until now, most research on these relationships has focused on comparing and contrasting these families to their more traditional counterpart, the nuclear family. As a result, how communication and relationships are conceptualized in these families has not yet reached the same level of theoretical sophistication as some of the other work reviewed in this chapter, and it also implies that the traditional family represents the standard of function and well-being (Koerner and Fitzpatrick 2013). The trend toward more complex theoretical explanations of family communication, however, suggests that our understanding of these relationships and the variables that are associated with them will increase dramatically in the future.

References

Afifi, Tamara D., Tara McManus, Susan Hutchinson and Birgita Baker. 2007. Parental divorce disclosures, the factors that prompt them, and their impact on parents’ and adolescents’ well-being. Communication Monographs 74: 78–103.

Altman, Irwin, and Dalmas Taylor. 1973. Social penetration: The development of interpersonal relationships. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston

Baumrind, Diana. 1967. Child care practices anteceding three patterns of preschool behavior. Genetic Psychology Monographs 75: 43–88.

Baumrind, Diana. 1971. Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology 4, 1–103.

Baumrind, Diana, Robert E. Larzelere and Philip A. Cowan. 2002. Ordinary physical punishment: Is it harmful? Comment on Gershoff (2002). Psychological Bulletin 128: 580–589.

Baxter, Leslie A., and Catherine L. Clark. 1996. Perceptions of family communication patterns and the enactment of family rituals. Western Journal of Communication 60: 254–268.

Braithwaite, Dawn O., and Leslie A. Baxter (eds.). 2006. Engaging theories in family communication: Multiple perspectives. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Brown, Penelope, and Stephen C. Levinson. 1987. Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Burleson, Brant R., Jesse G. Delia and James L. Applegate. 1995. The socialization of person-centered communication: Parents’ contributions to their children’s social-cognitive and communication skills. In: Mary Anne Fitzpatrick and Anita L. Vangelisti (eds.), Explaining family interactions, 34–76. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Campione-Barr, Nicole and Judith G. Smetana. 2010. “Who said you could wear my sweater?”: Adolescent siblings’ conflicts and associations with relationship quality. Child Development 81: 464–471.

Carr, Jen L. and Karen M. VanDeusen. 2002. The relationship between family of origin violence and dating violence in college men. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 17: 630–646.

Caughlin, John P. 2010. A multiple goals theory of personal relationships: Conceptual integration and program overview. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 27: 824–848.

Caughlin, John P., Sandra Petronio and Ashley V. Middleton. 2013. When families manage private information. In: Anita L. Vangelisti (ed.) The Routledge handbook of family communication, (2nd ed.), 321–337. New York: Routledge.

Caughlin, John P. and Marshall A. Scott. 2010. Toward a communication theory of the demand/ withdraw pattern of interaction in interpersonal relationships. In: Sandi W. Smith and Steve R. Wilson (eds.), New directions in interpersonal communication, 180–200. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Caughlin, John P., and Anita L. Vangelisti. 2006. Conflict in dating and marital relationships. In: John G. Oetzel and Stella Ting-Toomey (eds.), Sage handbook of conflict communication: Integrating theory, research, and practice, 129–157. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Clark, Margaret S., Julie Fitness and Ian Brissette. 2001. Understanding people’s perceptions of relationships is crucial to understanding their emotional lives. In: Garth J. O. Fletcher and Margaret S. Clark (eds.), Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Interpersonal Processes, 253–278. Malden, MA: Blackwell.

Collins, Andrew W. and Coral Luebker. 1994. Parent and adolescent expectancies: Individual and relationship significance. In: Judith G. Smetana (ed.), Beliefs about parenting: Origins and developmental implications. New Directions for Child Development (No. 66), 65–80. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Cummings, Mark E., Ronald J. Ianotti and Carolyn Zahn-Waxler. 1985. The influence of conflict between adults on the emotion and aggression in young children. Developmental Psychology 21: 495–507.

Donohue, William A., and Robert Kolt. 1992. Managing interpersonal conflict. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Elwood, Terrisa D. and David C. Schrader. 1998. Family communication patterns and communication apprehension. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality. 13: 493–502

Finkelhor, David, Heather A. Turner and Sherry L. Hamby. 2005. The victimization of children and youth: A comprehensive, national survey. Child Maltreatment 10: 5–25.

Fitness, Julie. 2013. The communication of emotion in families. In: Anita L. Vangelisti (ed.) The Routledge handbook of family communication (2nd ed.), 377–390. New York: Routledge.

Fitzpatrick, Mary Anne and Ascan F. Koerner. 2005. Family communication schemata: Effects on children’s resiliency. In: Dunwoody, Sharon., Lee B. Becker, Douglas M. McLeod, and Gerald M. Kosicki (eds.), The evolution of key mass communication concepts: Honoring Jack M. McLeod, 115–139. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.

Fitzpatrick, Mary Anne and David L. Ritchie. 1994. Communication schemata within the family: Multiple perspectives on family interaction. Human Communication Research 20: 275–301.

Floyd, Kory. 2001. Human affection exchange I: Reproductive probability as a predictor of men’s affection with their sons. Journal of Men’s Studies 10: 39–50.

Floyd, Kory and Mark T. Morman. 2001. Human affection III: Discriminative parental solicitude in men’s affectionate communication with their biological and nonbiological sons. Communication Quarterly 49: 310–327.

Floyd, Kory and Mark T. Morman. 2003. Human affection exchange II: Affectionate communication in father-son relationships. Journal of Social Psychology 143: 599–612.

Floyd, Kory and Mark T. Morman. 2005. Fathers’ and sons’ reports of fathers’ affectionate communication: Implications of a naive theory of affection. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 22: 99–109.

Floyd, Kory, Jack E. Sargent, and Mark Di Corcia. 2004. Human affection exchange VI: Further tests of reproductive probability as a predictor of men’s affection with their adult sons. The Journal of Social Psychology 144: 191–206.

Gershoff, Elizabeth T. 2002. Corporal punishment by parents and associated child behaviors and experiences: A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin 128: 539–579.

Gil-González, Diana, Carmen Vives-Cases, María T. Ruiz, Mercedes Carrasco-Portiño and Carlos Álvarez-Dardet. 2008. Childhood experiences of violence in perpetrators as a risk factor of intimate partner violence: A systematic review. Journal of Public Health 30: 14–22.

Gordis, Elana B., Gayla Margolin and Richard S. John. 2001. Parents’ hostility in dyadic marital and triadic family settings and children’s behavior problems. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 69: 727–734.

Gottman, John M. 1991. Predicting the longitudinal course of marriages. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy 17: 3–7.

Gottman, John M. 1993. The roles of conflict engagement, escalation or avoidance in marital interaction: A longitudinal view of five types of couples. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 61: 6–15.

Hamby, Sherry, David Finkelhor, Heather Turner and Richard Ormrod. 2011. Children’s exposure to intimate partner violence and other family violence pgs. 1–12 Juvenile Justice Bulletin – NCJ 232272. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

Harris, Judith R. 1998. The nature assumption. New York: Free Press.

Holden, George W., Pamela C. Miller and Susan D. Harris. 1999. The instrumental side of corporal punishment: Parents’ reported practices and outcome expectancies. Journal of Marriage and the Family 61: 908–919.

Jenkins, Jennifer. 2000. Marital conflict and children’s emotions: The development of an anger organization. Journal of Marriage and the Family 62: 723–736.

Johnson, Michael P. and Kathleen J. Ferraro. 2000. Research on domestic violence in the 1990s: Making distinctions. Journal of Marriage and Family 62: 948–963.

Koerner, Ascan F. 2007. Social cognition and family communication. In: David R. Roskos-Ewoldson and Jennifer L. Monahan (eds.), Social Cognition and communication, 197–216. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Koerner, Ascan F. 2013. Family conflict communication. In: John Oetzel and Stella Ting-Toomey (eds.), The Sage handbook of conflict communication (2nd ed.), 211–235. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Koerner, Ascan F. and Mary Anne Fitzpatrick. 1997. Family type and conflict: The impact of conversation orientation and conformity orientation on conflict in the family. Communication Studies 48: 59–75.

Koerner, Ascan F. and Mary Anne Fitzpatrick. 2002a. Toward a theory of family communication. Communication Theory 12: 70–91.

Koerner, Ascan F. and Mary Anne Fitzpatrick. 2002b. Understanding family communication patterns and family functioning: The roles of conversation orientation and conformity orientation. Communication Yearbook 26: 37–69.

Koerner, Ascan F. and Mary Anne Fitzpatrick. 2002c. You never leave your family in a fight: The impact of family of origin on conflict behavior in romantic relationships. Communication Studies 53: 234–251.

Koerner, Ascan F. and Mary Anne Fitzpatrick. 2006a. Family communication patterns theory: A social cognitive approach. In: Dawn O. Braithwaite and Leslie A. Baxter (eds.), Engaging theories in family communication: Multiple perspectives, 50–65. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Koerner, Ascan F. and Mary Anne Fitzpatrick. 2006b. Family conflict communication. In: John Oetzel and Stella Ting-Toomey (eds.), The Sage handbook of conflict communication, 159–183. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Koerner, Ascan F. and Mary Anne Fitzpatrick. 2013. Communication in intact families. In: Anita L. Vangelisti (ed.), The Routledge handbook of family communication (2nd ed.), 129–144. New York: Routledge.

Koerner, Ascan F. and Laura Maki. 2004. May Family communication patterns and violence in families. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Communication Association, New Orleans.

Laursen, Brett. 1993. Conflict management among close peers. In: Brett Laursen (ed.), Close friendship in adolescents. New Directions for Child Development (No. 60), 39–54. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Laursen, Brett and W. Andrew Collins. 2004. Parent-child communication during adolescence. In: Anita L. Vangelisti (ed.), The handbook of family communication, 333–348. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Laursen, Brett, Katherine C. Coy, and W. Andrew Collins. 1998. Reconsidering changes in parent-child conflict across adolescence: A meta-analysis. Child Development 69: 817–832.

MacKenzie, Michael J., Eric Nicklas, Jane Waldfogel and Jeanne Brooks-Gunn. 2011. Corporal punishment and child behavioural and cognitive outcomes through 5 years of age: Evidence from a contemporary urban birth cohort study. Infant and Child Development 21: 3–33.

Montemayor, Raymond and Eric A. Hanson. 1985. A naturalistic view of conflict between adolescents and the parents and siblings. Journal of Early Adolescents 3, 83–103.

Morman, Mark T. and Kory Floyd. 2002. A “changing culture of fatherhood”: Effects on affectionate communication, closeness, and satisfaction in men’s relationships with their fathers and their sons. Western Journal of Communication 66: 395–411.

Morman, Mark T. and Kory Floyd. 2006. Good fathering: Father and son perceptions of what it means to be a good father. Fathering: A Journal of Theory, Research and Practice about Men as Fathers 4, 113–136.

Noller, Patricia. 1995. Parent-adolescent relationships. In: Mary Anne Fitzpatrick and Anita L. Vangelisti (eds.), Explaining family interactions, 77–111. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Olson, David H. 1981. Family typologies: Bridging family research and family therapy. In: Erik E. Filsinger and Robert A. Lewis (eds.), Assessing marriage: New behavioral approaches, 74–89. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Olson, David H. 1993. Circumplex model of marital and family systems: Assessing family functioning. In: Froma Walsh (ed.), Normal family processes (2nd ed.), 104–137. New York: Guilford Press.

Olson, David H., Candyce S. Russell and Douglas H. Sprenkle. 1979. Circumplex model of marital and family systems, VI: Theoretical update. Family Process 22: 69–83.

Olson, David H. Douglas H. Sprenkle and Candyce S. Russell. 1979. Circumplex model of marital and family systems: Cohesion and adaptability dimensions, family types, and clinical applications. Family Process 18: 3–28.

Palazzolo, Kellie E., Anthony J. Roberto and Elizabeth A. Babin. 2010. The relationship between parents’ verbal aggression and young adult children’s intimate partner violence victimization and perpetration. Health Communication 25: 357–364.

Petronio, Sandra. 2002. Boundaries of privacy: Dialectics of disclosure. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

Petronio, Sandra. 2010. Communication privacy management in families: What do we know? Journal of Family Theory and Review 2, 175–96.

Petronio, Sandra and John P. Caughlin. 2006. Communication privacy management theory: Understanding families. In: Dawn O. Braithwaite and Leslie A. Baxter (eds.), Engaging theories in family communication: Multiple perspectives, 35–49. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Reese-Weber, Maria and Suzanne Bertle-Haring. 1998. Conflict resolution styles in family subsystems and adolescent romantic relationships. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 27: 735–752.

Rhoades, Kimberly A. 2008. Children’s responses to interparental conflict: A meta-analysis of their associations with child adjustment. Child Development 79: 1942–1956.

Rinaldi, Christina M. and Nina Howe. 2003. Perceptions of constructive and destructive conflict within and across family subsystems. Infant and Child Development 12: 441–459.

Ritchie, L. David and May Anne Fitzpatrick. 1990. Family communication patterns: Measuring interpersonal perceptions of interpersonal relationships. Communication Research 17: 523– 544.

Roloff, Michael E. and Courtney W. Miller. 2006. Mulling about family conflict and communication: What we know and what we need to know. In: Lynn H. Turner and Richard West (eds.), Family communication: A reference of theory and research, 143–164. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Salzinger, Suzanne, Richard S. Feldman, Daisy S. Ng-Mak, Elana Mojica, Tanya Stockhammer and Margaret Rosario. 2002. Effects of partner violence and physical child abuse on child behavior: A study of abused and comparison children. Journal of Family Violence 17: 23–52.

Schrodt, Paul and Andrew M. Ledbetter. 2007. Communication processes that mediate family communication patterns and mental well-being: A mean and covariance structures analysis of young adults from divorced and non-divorced families. Human Communication Research 33: 330–356.

Schrodt, Paul, Andrew M. Ledbetter and Jennifer K. Ohrt. 2007. Parental confirmation and affection as mediators of family communication patterns and children’s mental well-being. Journal of Family Communication 7, 23–46.

Shantz, Carolyn U. and Cathy J. Hobart. 1989. Social conflict and development: Peers and siblings. In: Thomes J. Berndt and Gary W. Ladd (eds.), Peer relationships and child development, 71–94. New York: Wiley.

Sillars, Alan and Daniel J. Canary. 2013. Conflict and relational quality in families. In Anita L. Vangelisti (ed.), The Routledge handbook of family communication (2nd ed), 338–357. New York: Routledge.

Sillars, Alan, Daniel J. Canary and Melissa Tafoya. 2004. Communication, conflict, and the quality of family relationships. In: Anita L. Vangelisti (ed.), The handbook of family communication, 413–446. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Sillars, Alan, Ascan F. Koerner and Mary Anne Fitzpatrick. 2005. Communication and understanding in parent-adolescent relationships. Human Communication Research 31: 103– 128.

Simons, Ronald L., Kuei-Hsiu Lin and Leslie C. Gordon. 1998. Socialization in the family of origin and male dating violence: A prospective study. Journal of Marriage and the Family 60: 467– 478.

Smetana, Judith G., Jenny Yau and Shirley Hanson. 1991. Conflict resolution in families with adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence 1, 189–206.

Stamp, Glen H. and Carolyn K. Shue. 2013. Twenty years of family research published in communication journals: A review of the perspectives, theories, concepts, and contexts. In: Anita L. Vangelisti (ed.), The Routledge Handbook of family communication (2nd ed.), 11–28. New York: Routledge.

Steinberg, Laurence. 1990. Autonomy, conflict, and harmony in the family relationship. In: S. Shirley Feldman and Glen A. Elliot (eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent, 255–276. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Steinberg, Laurence. 2001. We know these things: Parent-adolescent relationships in retrospect and prospect. Journal of Research on Adolescence 11: 1–19.

Steinberg, Laurence, Julie Elmen and Nina Mounts. 1989. Authoritative parenting, psychosocial maturity, and academic success among adolescents. Child Development 60: 1424–1436.

Straus, Murray A. 1990. Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The conflict tactics (CT) scale. In: Murray. A. Straus and Richard J. Gelles (eds.), Physical violence in American families: Risk factors and adaptations to violence in 8,145 families, 29–47. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.

Straus, Murray A. 1994. Beating the devil out of them. New York: Lexington.

Straus, Murray A. and Richard J. Gelles. 1990. How violent are American families? Estimates from the national family violence resurvey and other studies. In: Murray A. Straus and Richard J. Gelles (eds.), Physical violence in American families: Risk factors and adaptations to violence in 8,145 families, 95–112. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers

Straus, Murray A. and Mallie J. Paschall. 2009. Corporal punishment by mothers and development of children’s cognitive ability: A longitudinal study of two nationally representative age cohorts. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment and Trauma 18: 459–483.

Straus, Murray A. and Carrie L. Yodanis. 1996. Corporal punishment in adolescence and physical assaults on spouses later in life: What accounts for the link? Journal of Marriage and the Family 58: 825–841.

Swinford, Steven P., Alfred DeMaris, Stephen A. Cernkovich and Peggy C. Giordano. 2000. Harsh physical discipline in childhood and violence in later romantic involvements: The mediating role of problem behaviors. Journal of Marriage and the Family 62: 508–519.

Tucker, Corinna J., Susan M. McHale and Ann C. Crouter. 2003. Conflict resolution: Links with adolescents’ family relationships and individual well being. Journal of Family Issues 24: 715–736.

Vangelisti, Anita L. 1994. Family secrets: Forms, functions and correlates. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 11: 113–135.

Vangelisti, Anita L. 1997. Revealing family secrets: The influence of topic, function, and relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 14: 679–705.

Vangelisti, Anita L. (ed). 2004. Handbook of family communication. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Vangelisti, Anita L. (ed). 2013. The Routledge handbook of family communication (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge.

Vangelisti, Anita L., John. P. Caughlin and Lindsay Timmerman. 2001. Criteria for Revealing Family Secrets. Communication Monographs 68: 1–27.

Whitfield, Charles L., Robert F. Anda, Shanta R. Dube and Vincent J. Felitti. 2003. Violent childhood experiences and the risk of intimate partner violence in adults. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 18: 166–185.

Yexley, Melinda, Iris Borowsky and Marjorie Ireland. 2002. Correlation between different experiences of intrafamilial physical violence and violent adolescent behavior. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 17: 707–720.

Young, Stacy L. 2009. The function of parental communication patterns: Reflection-enhancing and reflection-discouraging approaches. Communication Quarterly 47: 379–394.

..................Content has been hidden....................

You can't read the all page of ebook, please click here login for view all page.
Reset
3.136.234.229