Chapter 45
The Role of Embodiment in Optimal Functioning

KATE HEFFERON

The body may be a “marvel of nature,” but it is the one marvel of nature that we “least stop to observe” (Aldersey-Williams, 2013, p. xviii). This is no more so than within the area of positive psychology. Despite rapid growth and advancement over the past decade (Rusk & Waters, 2013; Wong, 2011), the body is still overlooked within positive psychology, which tends to be a “neck-up focused discipline” (Peterson, 2012; Seligman, 2008) preoccupied by cognitive and emotional phenomena. This lack of interest in the physical self has been a critique of positive psychology since its conception (Anderson, 2001; Resnick, Warmoth, & Serlin, 2001). Furthermore, most of what we do know about the body and its effects on well-being has been from a pathological perspective (e.g., anomalous bodily experiences, disorders of self-image; MacLachlan, 2004). Thus, the aim of this chapter is to introduce the concept of embodiment and to argue that positive psychology has an opportunity to go beyond this deficiency in research and practice toward a more holistic, embodied approach to human flourishing. Indeed, how we “treat our body; move our body; soothe our body; feed our body; dress our body; decorate our body and connect to our body, all have an impact on our hedonic and eudaimonic well being” (Hefferon, 2013, p. ix). First, I discuss the research area of embodied cognition and its effects on emotions and overall well-being. Second, I review research on the newly defined construct of positive body image (PBI) and how it impacts our subjective perceptions of flourishing. Finally, evidence-based embodied interventions that have been found to have a positive influence on our optimal human functioning are described.

Understanding Embodiment

Embodiment is an area of research that has been dissected across many disciplines ranging from sociology to cognitive neuroscience (Kontra, Goldin-Meadow, & Beilock, 2012). Embodiment has been defined in various ways, with the consensus being that it is “an awareness of and responsiveness to bodily sensations” (Impett, Daubenmier, & Hirschman, 2006, p. 40) as well as the belief that “thoughts, feelings and behaviours are grounded in sensory experiences and bodily states” (Meier, Schnall, Schwartz, & Bargh, 2012, p. 2). Understanding our body's sensations and signals is imperative for self-knowledge, enhancing positive emotional experiences and psychological well-being (Anderson, 2006; Impett et al., 2006).

There are two approaches to embodiment: simple and radical. Simple embodiment adheres to the more cognitive scientific view of the body, stating that although the body plays an important role, it is just another “add on feature” (Meier et al., 2012, p. 7). The radical view, however, challenges this computation perspective, stating that the body, the brain, and the outer world are complexly intertwined. Thus, all mental processes are grounded in bodily experiences, and it is not possible to remove the mental processing from the body and the body from the self—it is the means of perception (Merleau-Ponty, 1964).

Embodiment and Well-Being

Embodiment is important to positive psychology for two major reasons. First, the body and its physical state can have a significant effect on shaping our emotions, and second, the body plays an important role during social interactions and the perceived quality of relationships. We know this from the burgeoning field of embodied cognition that aims to assess how cognition is influenced by bodily states, actions, and the environment (Kontra et al., 2012) as well as how the perception of and thought processing of emotions involves “perceptual, somato-visceral and motor re-experiencing (collectively referred to as embodiment) of the relevant emotion in one's self” (Niedenthal, 2007, p. 1002). Thus, a significant amount of focus targets how people's body state (temperature, position, gestures, facial expressions) affects how they interpret their social and personal surroundings (e.g., interaction with others, emotions, etc.; Vacharkulksemsuk & Fredrickson, 2012).

To be sure, the area of hedonic well-being focuses heavily on the experience of positive emotions and their role in the pleasurable life (Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999). From the current research, there is evidence to support the role of the body (postures, gestures, and facial expressions) directly affecting the experience of positive mood, feelings of success, and achievement (Stepper & Strack, 1993; Strack, Martin, & Stepper, 1988) as well as anger (Chandler & Schwartz, 2009) and guilt (Rotella & Richeson, 2013; see Barsalou, Niedenthal, Barbey, & Ruppert, 2003). For example, manipulations of back posture during lab experiments (slumped versus straight) subsequently affected the interpretation of achievement scores such that individuals manipulated into straight condition reported better moods and more pride in their achievements than peers within the slumped category (Stepper & Strack, 1993). Researchers have also studied the links between physical and moral cleanliness (physical purity), with the act of washing one's hands reducing feelings of guilt and intensity of moral judgments (Schnall, Benton, & Harvey, 2008; Zhong & Liljenquist, 2006). Furthermore, embodiment can also influence the perception of duration of emotional experiences, such that individuals who are free to spontaneously imitate other persons' facial expressions (angry and happy) overestimate the duration of exposure to the stimuli relative to neutral stimuli (Effron, Niedenthal, Gil, & Droit-Volet, 2006).

The same embodied connections have been linked to simply thinking about emotions, such that when one is engaging in a task exposed to words that have direct and indirect associations with emotions, there is an immediate evoked embodied response within associated muscles, influencing subsequent personal judgments (Niedenthal, 2007; Niedenthal, Winkielman, Mondillon, & Vermeulen, 2009). The researchers argue that using stored emotional information requires embodiment. Furthermore, Foroni & Semin (2009) found increased activation in facial muscles when individuals read actions words for positive emotional expressions (e.g., smile) versus nonaction words (e.g., funny). These and several other body manipulation/emotion tasks have supported the argument that body states can have an influence on the subsequent attention and interpretation of emotions (Niedenthal, 2007).

Embodiment and Intrapersonal Relationships

Body awareness is one of the key components to embodiment and refers to one's ability to correctly and confidently identify and engage with body sensations as well as link these sensations to emotions (Mehling et al., 2009). Although an overlooked area in positive psychology, the importance of body awareness has been touted across several domains and should be an additional component to the flourishing agenda. Body awareness can be broken down into four dimensions, including perceived body sensation (ability to correctly identify sensations); attention quality (willingness to engage with these sensations versus overlook them); attitude (level of trust in correct identification of sensations and use of these for adaptive future decision making); and mind–body integration (level of connection between sensations and emotions and overall embodiment; Mehling et al., 2009). Currently, the Body Awareness Questionnaire (BAQ; Shields, Mallory, & Simon, 1989) is the most widely used measurement tool within research and practice. Another tool is the Body Intelligence Scale (BIS), which aims to measure three different forms of body awareness (energy, comfort, and inner) that potentially contribute to wellness (Anderson, 2006).

Traditionally, heightened body awareness and identification of bodily sensations has been associated with maladaptive functioning within chronic physical and mental illness (e.g., pain, depression, eating disorders; Mehling et al., 2009, 2011). Despite an earlier pathological focus on body awareness, a new wave of research indicates the benefits of body awareness in relation to both hedonic and eudaimonic well-being (Anderson, 2006; Mehling et al., 2009). For example, Brani, Hefferon, Lomas, Ivtzan, and Painter (in press) found a predictive relationship between individuals' self-reported body awareness and subjective well-being (SWB; Diener et al., 1999) within the normal population. This was one of the first findings that suggested that individuals could benefit from becoming more body aware as a potential route to SWB.

From a more eudaimonic perspective, research has shown a link between trauma, the realization of mortality, and heighted body awareness (Frank, 2002; Meier et al., 2012). For example, my own work into embodiment and posttraumatic growth (PTG) has shown that the body plays an important role in the process and facilitation of PTG (Hefferon, 2012, 2013; Hefferon, Grealy, & Mutrie, 2008–2010). PTG is the notion that through the struggle of adversity people may change in ways that propel them to higher levels of functioning (psychological, physical) than existed before the event occurred. Survivors can express several notable changes, including perceived changes in self; improved relationships; changed life philosophy with increased existential awareness; changed priorities; and enhanced spiritual beliefs (Calhoun, Tedeschi, Cann, & Hanks, 2010; see also Tedeschi, Calhoun, & Groleau, Chapter 30, this volume). Research shows that illness and physical trauma can lead to more corporeal awareness and the elimination of the taken-for-granted body (Frank, 1995; Hefferon, 2012; Hefferon et al., 2009, 2010). These corporeal circumstances can enable us to become more embodied and aware of our physical, psychological, and spiritual existence and awaken us to the here and now (Burkeman, 2012; Yalom, 2008). The mission for positive psychology is to learn how we can help individuals live more below their shoulders on an everyday basis, not only when trauma and illness occur.

In terms of future research and practice, positive psychologists could start to include measurement tools such as the BAQ (Shields, Mallory, & Simon, 1989) or BIS (Anderson, 2006) when researching interventions, (e.g., mindfulness meditation, yoga, etc.) as the development of interoceptive body awareness has been shown to improve via meditative practices (Mehling et al., 2009; Siegel, 2010). Similarly, those working in more therapeutic environments can take heed from practices such as body psychotherapy, which literally uses the body as a tool within its sessions, engaging in somatic exercises and encouraging the reconnection of body and mind.

Positive Body Image: A New Frontier for Positive Psychology

There is one relationship that can arguably impact our well-being the most, and that is the relationship we have with our body (Cash & Smolak, 2011). This intrapersonal relationship (a.k.a. personal body image) is a notoriously complex construct that encompasses “self-perceptions and attitudes (i.e. thoughts feelings and behaviours) with regards to the body” (Avalos, Tylka, & Wood-Barcalow, 2005, p. 285) as well as “how people experience their own embodiment” (Cash, 2008, p. 1). Body image consists of several dimensions, including:

  1. Perceptual—how the body is pictured in one's own mind.
  2. Affective—the feelings one has about the body's appearance and function (pride and happiness vs. anxiety and shame).
  3. Cognitive—attitudes and thoughts about one's body (value placed on image and investment in appearance).
  4. Behavioral—how one displays outwardly his or her relationship with his or her body (e.g., revealing clothes).
  5. Subjective satisfaction—one's overall and specific satisfaction with her or his body and body parts (Martin-Ginis, Bassett, & Conlin, 2012).

Historically, the area of body image research has taken a pathological approach, focusing on the alleviation of body dissatisfaction. Through decades of study, there is strong evidence to link the presence of a negative body image with increased incidences of eating disorders, higher rates of depression, negative affect, social anxiety, lower confidence in relationships, and lower overall psychological well-being (Avalos et al., 2005; Swami, Begum, & Petrides, 2010).

Despite this previous pathological preoccupation, body image has been found to have both negative and positive features, and the absence of a negative body image does not necessitate a positive body image (Tylka, 2011). For example, not hating your body is not the same as “appreciating, respecting, celebrating and honouring the body” (Tylka, 2011, p. 63). Williams, Cash, and Santos (2004) further demonstrated that body image could be divided into three types—negative, positive, and neutral—with those displaying higher levels of PBI reporting higher levels of psychological and social well-being (e.g., optimism, self-esteem, and adaptive coping). Ultimately, a PBI begins with love and respect for the body. From this foundation, individuals can begin to appreciate the uniqueness of their body and its functionality. PBI is about (1) accepting the body and all its imperfections; (2) being aware of and focusing on assets of the body rather than on the negatives (protective filtering); (3) having a mindful connection with the body and the body's needs; (4) making a conscious effort to absorb positive information and reject or reframe negative information (Tylka, 2011; Wood-Barcalow, Tylka, & Augustus-Horvath, 2010); and (5) feeling “beautiful, comfortable, confident and happy with their body” (Tylka, 2011, p. 58). Ultimately, people with a PBI are in tune with and mindful of their body's needs, listen to and respect their own bodies, and take care of them when needed.

There are several preconditions that have been identified as facilitators of PBI (Tylka, 2011), including (a) the experience of unconditional acceptance of who they are from loved ones; (b) a comprehension of media images (photo-shopping, lighting, etc.); (c) environments or cultures that broadly define beauty; (d) a belief in a higher power that creates all to be unique; and (e) surrounding themselves in a social circle that also has a PBI (Wood-Barcalow et al., 2010). The body appreciation scale (BAS) was developed to measure PBI and encompasses the aforementioned conditions, as well as including several other components, such as “(a) favourable opinions of the body (regardless of actual physical appearance); (b) acceptance of the body in spite of weight, body shape and imperfections; (c) respect of the body by attending to its needs and engaging in healthy behaviours; and (d) protection of the body by rejecting unrealistic body image portrayed in the media” (Avalos et al., 2005, p. 286). For young adolescents, PBI has been characterized by a functional view of the body and acceptance of imperfections as well as engagement in physical activity for the purpose of experiencing positive emotions and health benefits (Frisén & Holmqvist, 2010). Further inquiry by the same researchers found that adolescents who expressed having a PBI viewed appearance ideals via the media with criticality, as well as holding a flexible view of beauty (Holmqvist & Frisén, 2012).

Individuals with higher levels of PBI have been found to report several positive outcomes, such as intuitive eating, higher levels of self-esteem and optimism, adaptive coping, and better sexual functioning (Tiggemann & McCourt, 2013; Williams et al., 2004). However, as the area is very new, the research has predominantly been restricted to younger, female populations. Thus, there have been mixed and inconsistent results regarding gender differences within PBI as well as the degree to which ethnicity and culture can influence PBI (Swami, Hadji-Michael, & Furnham, 2008; Webb, Warren-Findlow, Chou, & Adams, 2013).

Promoting Positive Body Image

Whilst recognizing that this area of research is still in its infancy, there are current suggestions from researchers on how to promote PBI within young adolescent and young adult populations. Ideally, successful programs should include a media literacy approach, where adolescents are taught lessons surrounding the tricks of the trade within media advertising, including airbrushing, photoshopping, external supports (e.g., tape, bras), and so on, as well as promoting critical discussion behind the motives of the media agencies. PBI programs can also consider using a feminist approach to ideals by providing adolescents with alternatives to the ideals of media advertising in order to enable a more flexible perception of beauty. This would also help adolescents to challenge the sexual objectification of the body and promote engagement in nonobjectifying activities (e.g., physical activity, yoga). Finally, administrators could include a focus on ways to enhance self-esteem and body image (including acceptance of weight and body shape) as well as strategies for healthy eating and exercise (McVey et al., 2010). Physical activity is also a fantastic way to become more embodied, increase positive body image, and enhance our subjective and psychological well-being (see Faulkner, Hefferon, & Mutrie, Chapter 12, this volume).

Within therapeutic environments, therapists can help individuals to not only eliminate their negative body image, but also enhance their positive body image via supporting the use of “protective filtering, interpersonal support, associating with and modelling others who are proud of their body and embracing an inclusive definition of beauty” (Tylka, 2011, p. 61). Furthermore, therapists should suggest activities that promote embodiment, body functionality, and body acceptance such as yoga or physical activity (LePage & Crowther, 2010; Tylka, 2011). It is important to note that people can still have bad body image days where they internalize negative information; however, it can be recorrected via the aforementioned facilities, particularly by reframing negative information.

As a final point, the future research between positive psychology and PBI is rife with possibility. For example, Swami et al. (2010) found a significant relationship between trait emotional intelligence (EI) scores and body appreciation in undergraduate females. More specifically, they found that the well-being factor (self-esteem, positive outlook, and subjective happiness) within the trait EI measurement tool significantly predicted body appreciation scores and actual-ideal weight discrepancy, suggesting that enhancing well-being may also enhance body appreciation. Furthermore, researchers could also employ the use of positive psychological interventions (PPIs; Sin & Lyubomirksy, 2009) to enhance PBI. For example, Geraghty, Wood, and Hyland (2010) found that the use of a daily gratitude diary for 2 weeks was as effective as the treatment as usual (monitoring and reframing) group in decreasing body dissatisfaction. Future research could test different PPIs or contextualize the PPIs to focus on enhancing PBI rather than simply decreasing body dissatisfaction. Finally, Avalos et al. (2005) affirm that the PBI area would benefit from investigation into specific character strengths (Peterson & Seligman, 2004) that prevent body image disturbances and/or promote body appreciation.

Embodiment and Interpersonal Relationships

Not only are our bodies important for our intrapersonal relationships, they are crucial for creating and maintaining interpersonal connections. Over the past decade, one of the strongest findings from positive psychology is the relationship between social support, health, and overall well-being (Vacharkulksemsuk & Fredrickson, 2012). This connection has led to an increase in relationship science research (Fincham & Beach, 2010), which aims to understand how the quality of our interpersonal relationships (with both loved ones and strangers) influences our overall well-being. Embodied social cognition (e.g., simulation, mimicry, synchrony) enables us to not only think about what other humans might be thinking, but to actually experience their bodily states in order to give us a better insight into their intentions and needs (Cacioppo & Cacioppo, 2012). Embodied simulation is an area of research that claims that when we observe another carrying out actions, emotions, or even sensations (e.g., experiencing pain), the same associated bodily states are activated, creating an experience understanding of others (Gallese, 2006). For example, research has found that the same areas of the brain (anterior insula and anterior cingulate cortex) are activated when an individual personally experiences a disgusting smell as well as when she or he watches another person experiencing such a smell (Wicker et al., 2003). By physically simulating the experience, it is argued that we are better able to understand the intentions of others and better able to take the perspective of the person we are observing, thereby increasing levels of empathy. We are also able to feel the pain or touch of others, simply by watching others have the experience. This connection derived via embodied simulation can therefore help with the connection between strangers and other individuals.

Rapport is another concept within relationship science that has been studied in order to understand quality of interactions; however, rapport as a bodily experience has been underrepresented. Embodied rapport is shared feelings not just shared feelings of positivity and mutuality, but of vitality and aliveness (which stem from shared movements; Vacharkulksemsuk & Fredrickson, 2012). Embodied rapport can be achieved via behavioral synchrony, defined as “coordination of movement that occurs between individuals during a social interaction, featuring similarity of (1) form, the manner and style of movements and (2) time, the temporal rhythm of movements” (Vacharkulksemsuk & Fredrickson, 2012, p. 399). Synchrony is an important feature of interaction as it can create a sense of oneness as well as compassion, cooperation affiliation, emotional support satisfaction, and even elevated pain thresholds (Niedenthal, 2007). Researchers have also found that behavioral synchrony can mediate the direct effect of self-disclosure among strangers and enhance interaction quality (Vacharkulksemsuk & Fredrickson, 2012).

One of the most recent (and embodied) progressions in the area of positive psychology and relationship science is Fredrickson's new theory on love and positivity resonance (Fredrickson, 2013). Following on from her early research into positive emotions, Fredrickson has ascertained that love is a supreme emotion (more so than gratitude, joy, etc.) and is vital for flourishing. Fredrickson's theory of love is not reflective of the mainstream understanding of love (e.g., as a committed, long-lasting bond between two people, usually spouses or family members or friends). Instead, Fredrickson proposes love from a bodily perspective—as an embodied way of being. Love is momentary experiences that change the physiological nature of the two individuals experiencing micro-moments. Love, therefore, consists of three elements:

  1. “A sharing of one or more positive emotions between you and another.”
  2. “A synchrony between you and the other person's biochemistry and behaviours.”
  3. “A reflective motive to invest in each other's well being that brings mutual care” (Fredrickson, 2013, p. 17).

Together, these three events are coined as positivity resonance, where individuals mirror each other and deepen the interpersonal resonance. However, in order for positivity resonance to occur, there must exist several preconditions, including perception of safety and sensory and temporal connection.

In sum, this theory of love is an embodied perspective—one in which your body and another body are imperative for connection to occur. Fredrickson argues that talking on the telephone, e-mailing, or not being in the physical presence of the other person inhibits love from occurring. From an applied perspective, preliminary research findings have linked the effects of loving kindness meditation interventions to increased experiences of positive emotions and physical health (e.g., increased activation of the vagus nerve; Kok et al., 2013; Kok, Waugh, & Fredrickson, 2013). Ultimately, this new area is among the first positive psychological theories to prioritize the body as a main mechanism for well-being.

Becoming Embodied: Practical Applications

There are wide individual differences in how much people are aware of their bodies (Nettleton & Watson, 1998). There are many evidence-based practices that can be used to enhance people's connections to their bodies (see Table 45.1). Furthermore, the main evidence-based PPIs—gratitude (letter, counting blessings), random acts of kindness, cultivating strengths, visualizing best possible selves, and meditation (mindfulness, loving kindness)—can be manipulated to incorporate more corporeal elements to them, thereby providing more opportunity to enhance personality/activity fit (see Table 45.2 for theoretical manipulations and suggestions). Offering individuals variety and choice, from a suite of both cognitive and physical (embodied) interventions, will help support the person in sustaining his or her efforts to attain and maintain well-being (Lyubomirksy & Layous, 2013). Furthermore, with current positive psychology research implementation findings proclaiming the use of varied and multimodal methods of delivery, there is a tremendous amount of potential for a new wave of embodied positive psychological interventions (Layous, Nelson, & Lyubomirksy, 2012).

Table 45.1 Body-Based Interventions (see Hefferon, 2013)

Body-Based Interventions Overview of Research and Applications
Body awareness therapies
  • Enhancing a person's body awareness is at the heart of mind–body therapies (Mehling et al., 2011).
  • Body psychotherapy posits that the body itself can be used as a tool for healing and well-being.
  • Body awareness therapies use several body-based interventions (e.g., body diary) so clients can learn to make friends with their body, emotions, and associated sensations (Rothschild, 2000).
Physical activity
  • Physical activity is a fantastic way to become more embodied, increase positive body image, and enhance our subjective and psychological well-being (see Faulkner, Hefferon, & Mutrie, Chapter 12, this volume).
  • Physical activity should be incorporated into most well-being programs in order to create a more embodied approach to flourishing.
Yoga
  • Yoga is one of the most renowned mind–body exercises, which promotes engagement with body sensations and awareness (Impett et al., 2006).
  • Engagement can increase positive mood, energy, satisfaction, confidence, mindfulness, self-compassion, sleep quality, and overall quality of life.
  • Yoga can also decrease stress, anxiety, anger, depression, and back pain and reduce the experience of symptoms of clinical treatments (e.g., cancer; Field, 2011).
  • Increased frequency of yoga engagement increases levels of embodiment (body awareness + body responsiveness), positive affect, and satisfaction with life and reduces negative affect and body objectification (Impett et al., 2006).
  • Embodiment may be a key mediator between yoga and well-being.
Mindfulness
  • Mindfulness-based interventions have also been at the forefront of embodiment therapies and can enhance PBI (Cash, 2008).
  • Mindfulness aims to connect the body and the mind, using several body-based techniques in order to reassociate people with their bodily sensations (Hanson & Mendius, 2009).
  • The body scan asks individuals find a quiet place and run through a body scan of sensations, temperatures, and pressures throughout the body.
  • The purpose of the body scan is not necessarily for relaxation, but to heighten the connection and attention to the body and its sensations.
Progressive muscle relaxation (PMR)
  • PMR is a well-studied and utilized form of relaxation training that involves the systematic tensing and relaxation of isolated muscles over a period of time (Manzoni, Pagnini, Castelnuovo, & Molinari, 2008).
  • The aim is to learn how to relax tensed muscles that can be associated with anxiety (Jacobsen, 1938).
  • The process is similar to the body scan except instead of simply noting sensations, individuals are asked to tense a chosen muscle for approximately 10 seconds and then relax for 20 seconds.
  • They are then asked to note the difference in body sensation, from tense to relaxed, and then repeat on all major muscle groups.
Touch
  • As embodied humans, touch with and by others is imperative for optimal functioning (Gallace, 2012; Weiss, Wilson, & Morrison, 2004).
  • Society is becoming less and less tactile, perpetuating a sense of touch hunger, which is arguably having a very real, negative effect (Field, 2001).
  • Touch, whether from strangers or loved ones, can influence people's perceptions of trust, intimacy, and overall relational interactions (Gallace, 2012).
  • Individuals derive a great deal of joy and contentment, as well as reduced stress, from physical interaction with others (Grewen, Anderson, Girdler, & Light, 2003; Grewen, Girdler, Amico, & Light, 2005; Light, Grewen, & Amico, 2005).
  • Researchers are now focusing on the role of oxytocin (a peptide molecule found in the brain and released when touched), well-being, and social bonding.
  • Higher levels of oxytocin have been linked to enhanced trust and emotional support within couples as well as increased positive affect and reduced stress, fear, and anxiety (Carter, 2009; Rasmussen & Pressman, 2009).
Nutrition
  • The role of diet and nutrition in optimal functioning has been overlooked within positive psychology.
  • Diets full of whole foods (e.g., vegetable, fish, fruits) have been found to predict lower levels of depression 5 years later (Akbaraly et al., 2009) and Mediterranean diets have been found to enhance energy, vigor, and contentment, even after short interventions (10 days; McMillan, Owen, Kras, & Scholey, 2011).
  • Blanchflower, Oswald, and Stewart-Brown (2012) reported a link between well-being (across a range of measures) and the consumption of fruit and vegetables, indicating seven pieces as the optimal portion size.
  • White, Horwath, and Conner (2013) conducted a microlongitudinal, correlational design using diaries to assess the effects of diet on positive and negative affect on young adults.
  • Participants reported higher positive affect on the days where they ate more fruits and vegetables and this predicted their positive mood the following day.
  • Similar to Blanchflower et al. (2012), they reported that seven to eight servings were the optimal dosage for notable change.

Table 45.2 Embodied Adaptations to the Main Positive Psychological Interventions

Intervention Embodied Adaptation
Gratitude For 1 week, individuals are asked to write down three things that they are grateful for in relation to body functionality (what their body did well). If desired, participants can reflect on other aspects they are grateful for that are connected to PBI (e.g., support system, uniqueness; see Holmqvist & Frisén, 2012).
Best Possible Self Individuals can adapt this writing exercise in many ways. First, they can focus on writing about a best possible embodied self (e.g., future aspirations about activity levels, health, body awareness, etc.). Second, participants can engage in this exercise by utilizing embodied writing techniques, which challenge participants to write from the inside out (see Anderson, 2001).
Random Acts of Kindness On 1 day this week, individuals are asked to commit five different random acts of kindness where they explicitly set up the environment for the possibility of positivity resonance (see Fredrickson, 2013).
Character Strengths For 1 week, individuals are asked to use their top five strengths in order to manipulate their physical activity routine. For example, if a top strength is love and be loved, they can try to join an exercise class with a friend, thereby supporting their need for connection. If one of the top strengths is spirituality, they can move their activity outdoors to be closer to nature (see Hefferon & Mutrie, 2012).
Mindfulness This exercise asks participants to move away from viewing their body as something to objectify. Over the next week, individuals are asked to take 10 minutes out to engage in a body scan, connecting sensations to emotions and increasing body awareness (see Cash, 2008).

Conclusion

In conclusion, the aim of the chapter was to introduce embodiment research and urge positive psychologists to reconceptualize wellness beyond the cognitive and emotional to include the body. Positive psychology is defined as the “scientific study of virtue, meaning, resilience and well being, as well as evidence based applications to improve the life of individual's and society in the totality of life” (Wong, 2011, p. 72). The task of improving the lives of individuals becomes unfeasible if those we are trying to understand do not and cannot make sense of and connect to their own bodies. Areas of research such as embodied cognition, positive body image, and body-based interventions redirect the focus of positive psychology from a neck-up discipline to a more holistic and embodied approach.

Summary Points

  • The body plays a crucial role in our ability to experience both hedonic and eudaimonic well-being.
  • It is essential for positive psychology to start thinking about well-being from a wider perspective, one that includes the body not as an add-on feature but as part of the overall experience of well-being.
  • Embodiment can be integrated in many current positive psychological topics, such as emotion research and relationship science.
  • PPIs should contain somatic and visceral components (e.g., body awareness), which will encourage an understanding of the body and its impact on well-being.
  • Traditional PPIs can be adapted from their original structure to include more embodied features.
  • Additional research is needed into the mechanisms by which the body plays a role in the interpretation of emotions.
  • Further neurological research is needed to understand the connections between embodied social cognition and emotions (e.g., empathy).
  • Research is needed within the area of embodiment after trauma and the link between the taken-for-granted body, corporeal awareness, and gratitude.

References

  1. Akbaraly, T. N., Brunner, E. J., Ferrie, J. E., Marmot, M. G., Kivimaki, M., & Singh-Manoux, A. (2009). Dietary pattern and depressive symptoms in middle age. British Journal of Psychiatry, 195(5), 408–413.
  2. Aldersey-Williams, H. (2013). Anatomies: The human body, its parts and the stories they tell. London, UK: Penguin Press.
  3. Anderson, R. (2001). Embodied writing and reflections on embodiment. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 33(2), 83–98.
  4. Anderson, R. (2006). Body intelligence scale: Defining and measuring the intelligence of the body. The Humanistic Psychologist, 34(4), 357–367.
  5. Avalos, L., Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. (2005). The body appreciation scale: Development and psychometric evaluation. Body Image, 2(3), 285–297.
  6. Barsalou, L. W., Niedenthal, P. M., Barbey, A. K., & Ruppert, J. A. (2003). Social embodiment. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 43, 43–92.
  7. Blanchflower, D. G., Oswald, A. J., & Stewart-Brown, S. (2012). Is psychological well-being linked to the consumption of fruit and vegetables? Social Indicators Research, 1–17.
  8. Brani, O., Hefferon, K., Lomas, T., Ivtzan, I. & Painter, J. (in press). The impact of body awareness on subjective wellbeing: The role of mindfulness. International Journal of Body Psychotherapy.
  9. Burkeman, O. (2012). The antidote: Happiness for people who can't stand positive thinking. London, England: Cannongate.
  10. Cacioppo, S., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2012). Decoding the invisible forces of social connections. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 1–7.
  11. Calhoun, L. G., Tedeschi, R. G., Cann, A., & Hanks, E. A. (2010). Positive outcomes following bereavement: Paths to posttraumatic growth. Psychologica Belgica, 50 (1–2), 125–143.
  12. Carter, S. (2009). Oxytocin. In S. Lopez (Ed.), The encyclopedia of positive psychology (pp. 38–40). Chichester, England: Blackwell.
  13. Cash, T. F. (2004). Body image: Past, present, and future. Body Image, 1(1), 1–5.
  14. Cash, T. F. (2008). The body image workbook: An 8-step program for learning to like your looks (2nd ed.). Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
  15. Cash, T. F., & Smolak, L. (Eds.). (2011). Body image: A handbook of science, practice, and prevention (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  16. Chandler, J., & Schwarz, N. (2009). How extending your middle finger affects your perception of others: Learned movements influence concept accessibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(1), 123–128.
  17. Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125(2), 276.
  18. Effron, D. A., Niedenthal, P. M., Gil, S., & Droit-Volet, S. (2006). Embodied temporal perception of emotion. Emotion, 6(1), 1–9.
  19. Field, T. (2001). Touch. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  20. Field, T. (2011). Yoga clinical research review. Complementary Therapies in Clinical Practice, 17(1), 1–8.
  21. Fincham, F. D., & Beach, S. R. (2010). Of memes and marriage: Toward a positive relationship science. Journal of Family Theory & Review, 2(1), 4–24.
  22. Foroni, F., & Semin, G. R. (2009). Language that puts you in touch with your bodily feelings: The multimodal responsiveness of affective expressions. Psychological Science, 20(8), 974–980.
  23. Frank, A. W. (1995). The wounded storyteller: Body, illness and ethics. London, England: University of Chicago Press.
  24. Frank, A. W. (2002). At the will of the body: Reflections on illness. New York, NY: Mariner Books.
  25. Fredrickson, B. L. (2013). Love: 2.0. New York, NY: Hudson Street Press.
  26. Frisén, A., & Holmqvist, K. (2010). What characterizes early adolescents with a positive body image? A qualitative investigation of Swedish girls and boys. Body Image, 7(3), 205–212.
  27. Gallace, A. (2012). Living with touch. The Psychologist, 25(12), 896–899.
  28. Gallese, V. (2006). Embodied simulation: From mirror neuron systems to interpersonal relations. In Novartis Foundation (Ed.), Empathy and fairness (pp. 3–12). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
  29. Geraghty, A. W., Wood, A. M., & Hyland, M. E. (2010). Attrition from self-directed interventions: Investigating the relationship between psychological predictors, intervention content and dropout from a body dissatisfaction intervention. Social Science & Medicine, 71(1), 30–37.
  30. Grewen, K. M., Anderson, B. J., Girdler, S. S., & Light, K. C. (2003). Warm partner contact is related to lower cardiovascular reactivity. Behavioral Medicine, 29(3), 123–130.
  31. Grewen, K. M., Girdler, S. S., Amico, J., & Light, K. C. (2005). Effects of partner support on resting oxytocin, cortisol, norepinephrine, and blood pressure before and after warm partner contact. Psychosomatic Medicine, 67(4), 531–538.
  32. Hanson, R., & Mendius, R. (2009). Buddha's brain: The practical neuroscience of happiness, love, and wisdom. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
  33. Hefferon, K. (2012). Bringing back the body into positive psychology: The theory of corporeal posttraumatic growth in breast cancer survivorship. Psychology, 3, 12, 1238–1242.
  34. Hefferon, K. (2013). Positive psychology and the body: The somatopsychic side to flourishing. London, England: McGraw-Hill.
  35. Hefferon, K., Grealy, M., & Mutrie, N. (2008). The perceived influence of an exercise class intervention on the process and outcomes of posttraumatic growth. Journal of Mental Health and Physical Activity, 1(2), 47–88.
  36. Hefferon, K., Grealy, M., & Mutrie, N. (2009). Posttraumatic growth and life threatening physical illness: A systematic review of the qualitative literature. British Journal of Health Psychology, 14(2), 343–378.
  37. Hefferon, K., Grealy, M., & Mutrie, N. (2010). Transforming from cocoon to butterfly: The potential role of the body in the process of posttraumatic growth. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 50(2), 224–247.
  38. Hefferon, K., & Mutrie, N. (2012). Physical activity as a “stellar” positive psychology intervention. In E. Acevedo (Ed.), Oxford handbook of exercise psychology (pp. 117–128). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  39. Holmqvist, K., & Frisén, A. (2012). “I bet they aren't that perfect in reality”: Appearance ideals viewed from the perspective of adolescents with a positive body image. Body Image, 9(3), 388–395.
  40. Impett, E. A., Daubenmier, J. J., & Hirschman, A. L. (2006). Minding the body: Yoga, embodiment, and well-being. Sexuality Research & Social Policy, 3(4), 39–48.
  41. Jacobson, E. (1938). Progressive relaxation. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
  42. Kok, B. E., Coffey, K. A., Cohn, M. A., Catalino, L. I., Vacharkulksemsuk, T., Algoe, S. B., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2013). How positive emotions build physical health perceived positive social connections account for the upward spiral between positive emotions and vagal tone. Psychological Science, 24(7), 1123–1132.
  43. Kok, B. E., Waugh, C. E., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2013). Meditation and health: The search for mechanisms of action. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7(1), 27–39.
  44. Kontra, C., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Beilock, S. L. (2012). Embodied learning across the life span. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 731–739.
  45. Layous, K., Nelson, S. K., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2012). What is the optimal way to deliver a positive activity intervention? The case of writing about one's best possible selves. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14(12), 1–20.
  46. LePage, M., & Crowther, J. (2010). The effects of exercise on body satisfaction and affect. Body Image, 7, 124–130.
  47. Light, K. C., Grewen, K. M., & Amico, J. A. (2005). More frequent partner hugs and higher oxytocin levels are linked to lower blood pressure and heart rate in premenopausal women. Biological Psychology, 69(1), 5–21.
  48. Lyubomirsky, S., & Layous, K. (2013). How do simple positive activities increase well-being? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(1), 57–62.
  49. MacLachlan, M. (2004). Embodiment: Clinical, critical and cultural perspectives on health and illness. New York, NY: Open University Press.
  50. Manzoni, G., Pagnini, F., Castelnuovo, G., & Molinari, E. (2008). Relaxation training for anxiety: A ten years systematic review with meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry, 8, 41.
  51. Martin-Ginis, K., Bassett, R., & Conlin, C. (2012). Body image and exercise. In E. Acevedo (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of exercise psychology (pp. 55–75). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  52. McMillan, L., Owen, L., Kras, M., & Scholey, A. (2011). Behavioural effects of a 10-day Mediterranean diet. Appetite, 56(1), 143–147.
  53. McVey, G. L., Kirsh, G., Maker, D., Walker, K. S., Mullane, J., Laliberte, M.,…Banks, L. (2010). Promoting positive body image among university students: A collaborative pilot study. Body Image, 7(3), 200–204.
  54. Mehling, W. E., Gopisetty, V., Daubenmier, J., Price, C. J., Hecht, F. M., & Stewart, A. (2009). Body awareness: construct and self-report measures. PLoS One, 4(5), e5614.
  55. Mehling, W. E., Wrubel, J., Daubenmier, J. J., Price, C. J., Kerr, C. E., Silow, T.,…Stewart, A. L. (2011). Body Awareness: A phenomenological inquiry into the common ground of mind-body therapies. Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine, 6(1), 1–12.
  56. Meier, B. P., Schnall, S., Schwarz, N., & Bargh, J. A. (2012). Embodiment in social psychology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 705–716.
  57. Merleau-Ponty, M. (1964). The phenomenology of perception. New York, NY: Routledge.
  58. Nettleton, S., & Watson, J. (1998). The body in everyday life. London, England: Routledge.
  59. Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science, 316(5827), 1002–1005.
  60. Niedenthal, P. M., Winkielman, P., Mondillon, L., & Vermeulen, N. (2009). Embodiment of emotion concepts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(6), 1120.
  61. Peterson, C. (2012). Pursuing the good life: 100 reflections on positive psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  62. Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. (2004). Character strengths and virtues: A handbook and classification. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  63. Rasmussen, H., & Pressman, S. (2009). Physical health. In S. Lopez (Ed.), The encyclopedia of positive psychology (pp. 695–701). Chichester, England: Blackwell.
  64. Resnick, S., Warmoth, A., & Serlin, I. A. (2001). The humanistic psychology and positive psychology connection: Implications for psychotherapy. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 41(1), 73–101.
  65. Rotella, K. N., & Richeson, J. A. (2013). Body of guilt: Using embodied cognition to mitigate backlash to reminders of personal & ingroup wrongdoing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 643–650.
  66. Rothschild, B. (2000). The body remembers. New York, NY: Norton.
  67. Rusk, R. D., & Waters, L. E. (2013). Tracing the size, reach, impact, and breadth of positive psychology. Journal of Positive Psychology, 8(3), 207–221.
  68. Schnall, S., Benton, J., & Harvey, S. (2008). With a clean conscience cleanliness reduces the severity of moral judgments. Psychological Science, 19(12), 1219–1222.
  69. Siegel, D. (2010). Mindsight: The new science of personal transformation. New York, NY: Random House.
  70. Seligman, M. E. (2008). Positive health. Applied Psychology, 57(s1), 3–18.
  71. Shields, S. A., Mallory, M. E., & Simon, A. (1989). The body awareness questionnaire: Reliability and validity. Journal of Personality Assessment, 53(4), 802–815.
  72. Sin, N. L., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2009). Enhancing well-being and alleviating depressive symptoms with positive psychology interventions: A practice-friendly meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65, 467–487.
  73. Stepper, S., & Strack, F. (1993). Proprioceptive determinants of emotional and nonemotional feelings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(2), 211.
  74. Strack, F., Martin, L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions in the human smile: A non-obtrusive test of facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 768–777.
  75. Swami, V., Begum, S., & Petrides, K. V. (2010). Associations between trait emotional intelligence, actual–ideal weight discrepancy, and positive body image. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(5), 485–489.
  76. Swami, V., Hadji-Michael, M., & Furnham, A. (2008). Personality and individual difference correlates of positive body image. Body Image, 5(3), 322–325.
  77. Tiggemann, M., & McCourt, A. (2013). Body appreciation in adult women: Relationships with age and body satisfaction. Body Image, 10(4), 624–627.
  78. Tylka, T. L. (2011). Positive psychology perspectives on body image. Body image: A handbook of science, practice, and prevention (2nd ed., pp. 56–64). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  79. Vacharkulksemsuk, T., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2012). Strangers in sync: Achieving embodied rapport through shared movements. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(1), 399–402.
  80. Webb, J. B., Warren-Findlow, J., Chou, Y. Y., & Adams, L. (2013). Do you see what I see? An exploration of inter-ethnic ideal body size comparisons among college women. Body Image, 10(3), 369–379.
  81. Weiss, W., Wilson, P., & Morrison, D. (2004). Maternal tactile stimulation and the neurodevelopment of low birth weight infants. Infancy, 5, 85–107.
  82. White, B. A., Horwath, C. C., & Conner, T. S. (2013). Many apples a day keep the blues away—Daily experiences of negative and positive affect and food consumption in young adults. British Journal of Health Psychology, 18, 782–798.
  83. Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J. P., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in my insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron, 40(3), 655–664.
  84. Williams, E. F., Cash, T. F., & Santos, M. T. (2004). Positive and negative body image: Precursors, correlates and consequences. Paper presented at the 38th annual meeting of the Association for Advancement of Behaviour Therapy, New Orleans, LA.
  85. Wong, P. T. (2011). Positive psychology 2.0: Towards a balanced interactive model of the good life. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 52(2), 69–81.
  86. Wood-Barcalow, N. L., Tylka, T. L., & Augustus-Horvath, C. L. (2010). “But I like my body”: Positive body image characteristics and a holistic model for young-adult women. Body Image, 7(2), 106–116.
  87. Yalom, I. (2008). Staring at the sun: Overcoming the terror of death. London, England: Piatkus Books.
  88. Zhong, C. B., & Liljenquist, K. (2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313(5792), 1451–1452.
..................Content has been hidden....................

You can't read the all page of ebook, please click here login for view all page.
Reset
3.148.144.100